Sensory Integration parte 01

 

























































































































































Sensory

Integration

 Theory and Practice

Anita C. Bundy ScD, OT/L, FAOTA, FOTARA

 Professor and Department Head,

Occupational Therapy

 College of Health & Human Services

 Department of Occupational Therapy

 Colorado State University

 Fort Collins, CO

 Honorary Professor

 Occupational Therapy

 Faculty of Health Sciences

 University of Sydney

 Sydney, Australia

Shelly J. Lane PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA, CSU

 Professor, Occupational Therapy

 College of Health & Human Services

 Department of Occupational Therapy

 Colorado State University

 Fort Collins, CO

 Professor and Discipline Lead

 Discipline of Occupational Therapy

 School of Health Sciences

 University of Newcastle, Australia

 Associate Editors

Shelley Mulligan PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Associate Professor and Chairperson

 Department of Occupational Therapy

 University of New Hampshire

Stacey Reynolds PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Associate Professor

 Department of Occupational Therapy

 College of Health Professions

 Virginia Commonwealth University

THIRD EDITION

 F. A. Davis Company

 1915 Arch Street

 Philadelphia, PA 19103

 www.fadavis.com

 Copyright © 2020 by F. A. Davis Company

 Copyright © 2020 by F. A. Davis Company. All rights reserved. This product is protected by copyright. No part

of it may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic,

mechanical, photocopying, recording, or otherwise, without written permission from the publisher.

 Printed in the United States of America

 Last digit indicates print number: 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1

Acquisitions Editor: Christa Fratantoro

Director of Content Development: George W. Lang

Developmental Editor: Stephanie Kelly

Content Project Manager: Julie Chase

Art and Design Manager: Carolyn O’Brien

 As new scientifi c information becomes available through basic and clinical research, recommended treatments

and drug therapies undergo changes. The author(s) and publisher have done everything possible to make this

book accurate, up to date, and in accord with accepted standards at the time of publication. The author(s), editors,

and publisher are not responsible for errors or omissions or for consequences from application of the book, and

make no warranty, expressed or implied, in regard to the contents of the book. Any practice described in this book

should be applied by the reader in accordance with professional standards of care used in regard to the unique circumstances that may apply in each situation. The reader is advised always to check product information (package

inserts) for changes and new information regarding dose and contraindications before administering any drug.

Caution is especially urged when using new or infrequently ordered drugs.

 Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data

 Library of Congress Control Number:2019946075

 Authorization to photocopy items for internal or personal use, or the internal or personal use of specifi c clients, is

granted by F. A. Davis Company for users registered with the Copyright Clearance Center (CCC) Transactional

Reporting Service, provided that the fee of $.25 per copy is paid directly to CCC, 222 Rosewood Drive, Danvers,

MA 01923. For those organizations that have been granted a photocopy license by CCC, a separate system of

payment has been arranged. The fee code for users of the Transactional Reporting Service is: 978-0-8036-4606-

3/20 + $.25.

 The third edition of this book, similar to the second, is dedicated

 to A. Jean Ayres, the inspiration for this text

and the intervention and research it represents.

 And to those who live with sensory integrative dysfunction,

who are the best teachers.

vii

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

 Whenever the two of us edit a textbook, it seems

to involve moving around the world. This one

was no exception. This time each of us moved

to and/or from Australia (SJL moved both ways).

Those moves undoubtedly contributed to the time

it took for us to complete this edition—and also

makes us very grateful to a number of people,

without whom there would be no third edition.

 First, we are indebted to the therapists,

researchers, academics, and occupational therapy

students in many countries of the world who

read—and sometimes re-read—the fi rst two editions of this book. They tell us that those books

informed their practice—and they insisted we do

a third. This third edition is the last for us. We

are very grateful to Shelley Mulligan and Stacey

Reynolds, who joined us in editing this time. We

now pass the torch to them.

 It almost goes without saying that the contribution of each author was critical but we would

never want to be silent on this. This group of

scholars is amazing—and amazingly patient. We

are honored to have worked with them. We are

also thankful to the numerous therapists who

gave their time and resources to teach us about

Jean Ayres’ life and the history of sensory integration theory and practice. This information led

to Chapter 2 . Generous fi nancial contributions

from F. A. Davis—and a sabbatical from the

University of Sydney—allowed us to travel all

over the United States in search of their collective wisdom. We hope we have done a good job

of interpreting what they said.

 Tammie Fink, from the Sensory Gym in

Hobartville, New South Wales, Australia, is

responsible for many of the photographs in this

edition. The photos bring the content to life.

Our deep appreciation goes to Colleen Hacker

for sharing Tammie ’ s time, organizing children

and families for the photo shoot, and of course

getting those all-important photo releases signed.

We can ’ t even imagine a book without these

“action” photos.

 The reviewers of each chapter provided

invaluable feedback. What they had to say was

not always easy to hear—but it was always

correct. We hope we have done justice to their

comments.

 Numerous colleagues and friends contributed in enormously important ways by refl ecting

on content, terminology, and interpretations of

content. Judith Abelenda, in particular, not only

spent countless hours discussing ideas, but she

also made her home in Spain a writing retreat.

We made a lot of progress in that week.

 The folks at F.A. Davis stood by us through

thick and thin. Christa Fratantoro, in particular,

has been involved since the beginning and key

to the last two editions. She is always our “go-to

person” with any questions or needs. There are

so many others that it would be impossible to

list them without missing some. We are eternally

grateful to all for your support.

 Finally, as always, those closest to us, Rick

Thornton and Ginny Deal, literally provided

years of (nearly uncomplaining) support. Shelly ’ s

children, Hannah and Lucas Thornton, provided

her cheering section, something that kept her

going when deadlines loomed. We could not

have thanked them enough—and we ’ re pretty

sure they would say we did not.

 We have undoubtedly failed to mention some

key players. That has more to do with failed

memory than lack of signifi cance of their contributions. We hope you will forgive us.

 Anita Bundy

 Shelly Lane

ix

FOREWORD

 The rigor, passion, and vitality that A. Jean Ayres

brought to addressing the problems of sensory

integrative dysfunction in children is alive and

well as proven by this third edition of Bundy and

Lane ’ s Sensory Integration Theory and Practice.

Throughout the book, the authors exhibit current

work that fully understands and implements

contemporary education, research, and practice

agendas related to sensory integration (SI). Each

of the 23 chapters focuses on the importance of

discoveries that advance the initial theory and

application of SI fi rst presented by A. Jean Ayres

nearly 60 years ago.

 The legacy of A. Jean Ayres is steeped in a

tradition of inquiry and research. It is humbling

to realize the profound effect that Ayres’ foundational work still holds on the current day practice of occupational therapy. In the early 1960s,

she had a remarkable sense of how a deep and

detailed inquiry into neuroscience could provide

a bridge to the fi eld of occupational therapy when

applied to individuals with SI problems. Her

initial vision of what the marriage of these fi elds

could yield has resulted in a sustained effort of

thought and action that continues to propel us

toward a research agenda for the 21st century

( Chapters 15 and 16 ) and describe both assessment and intervention methodologies ( Chapters 8

to 13 ).

 Similar to Ayres herself, the authors in this

book are intrigued and driven by the complicated questions posed by SI dysfunction. Their

curiosity and good science expand her tradition of inquiry by asking diffi cult and probing

questions. Both the questions and the resultant

answers further inform the underlying theory

(Part I), explicate SI disorders and their neuroscience basis (Part II), explore the current status

of assessment and intervention (Parts III and IV),

extend theory application (Part V), and provide

case exemplars (Part VI).

 The authors collected together in this edited

text have made longstanding commitments to the

full realization of Ayres’ initial concepts and ideas

for approaching assessment and intervention.

Their chapters provide evidence of their ability

to think scientifi cally and synthesize knowledge

and understanding that readers will fi nd useful

as they defi ne and implement the next wave of

research. These authors exhibit the commitment

and courage refl ective of A. Jean Ayres. They continue the evolution of Ayres’ work, particularly

as evidenced in their consideration of broader

applications of the theory ( Chapter 16 ), inclusion of complementary approaches ( Chapter 17 ),

and application of SI principles beyond traditional diagnostic groups ( Chapter 18 ).

 The 1960s as a decade witnessed an explosion

of conceptual and theoretical work across a wide

swath of occupational therapy. Occupational

therapy thinkers sought to import relevant ideas

from neurobiology, kinesiology, psychoanalytic

theory, and the social sciences. Ayres’ contemporaries, including theoreticians such as Mary

Reilly, Gail Fidler, Anne Mosey, Margret Rood,

and Josephine Moore, to name a few, were eager

to develop models that would guide clinicians to

better assess and treat specifi c occupation-based

problems.

 These scholars recognized that occupational

therapy was maturing as a profession, shifting away from a dependency on medicine and

rehabilitation. Occupational therapy was ready

to stand on its own two feet. Focused attention

was needed to articulate theories with specifi c

concepts and principles that would sharpen the

understanding of the therapeutic value of an

occupation-based approach. This was to distinguish the focus of occupational therapy as

unique, and, as it turns out, lay the groundwork for what would become a research-based,

evidence-generating, highly valued and widely

 Janice P. Burke

 “There is no substitute for a good idea” ( Reynolds, 1971 ).

x ■ Foreword

recognized approach to remediating the problems

of occupation. Using strategies from the social

and hard sciences, they set out to understand and

solve problems of occupation and the diffi culties

of fully participating in society.

 Ayres, along with her contemporaries in neurobiology and education, were deeply involved

in identifying, defi ning, and understanding children with visual and perceptual motor problems

and learning diffi culties. During the 1960s, they

worked to develop ways for remediating occupational issues in both educational and clinical

settings.

 Ayres’ perspective was authentic to its occupational therapy roots. It had a sharp focus on

the needs of a special population of children and

was designed as a bridge on the road to full participation. It provided the fi eld with legitimate,

science-based explanations for the use of specifi c

occupational therapy approaches. Ayres drew the

attention of occupational therapists concerned

with children with neurologically based issues

to the possibilities of a theory rooted in scientifi cally grounded principles. This theory was

testable and subject to validation and proof. The

path Ayres forged was rigorous and would have

scholarly, and demanding, vistas.

 I had the great fortune of studying at the University of Southern California with two incredible

pioneers during this time of theoretical explosion

in occupational therapy: A. Jean Ayres and Mary

Reilly. Though their theoretical orientations were

different, Ayres in the neurosciences and Reilly

in social science, they both had a single-minded

perspective of their subject matter that led to an

unwavering commitment that set the stage for a

theory-driven approach to assessment and intervention. Both had a foundational belief in the

power of occupation. They employed similar

strategies, based in evidence and observation, to

test their ideas.

 They surrounded themselves with students and

scholars in graduate level education and worked

to engage them in a path to become great thinkers. In this way, A. Jean Ayres and Mary Reilly

planned to seed their ideas and create the next

generation of leaders. All the students in their

classrooms were able to benefi t from the natural

productivity that is inherent when one is curious,

a student, and learning at the table of wonderful

thinkers, mentors, and educators.

 Paul Reynolds in A Primer in Theory Construction, his book on theory, wrote: “[T]he ultimate test of any idea is its utility in achieving the

goals of science” ( Reynolds, 1971 ).

 I sat in graduate classes with both of them,

on mats in Jean ’ s clinic and at seminar tables

in Mary ’ s classroom. All present listened to the

exquisite ideas they were developing. There I

came to realize what was needed to reach the

infi nite possibilities and potentials that occupational therapy has to understand, articulate, and

remediate problems of individuals as they move

into a world of meaning and purpose.

 What is especially intriguing about this period

of theory development is to ask the question of

why some theories, such as that of A. Jean Ayres,

continued to evolve, prove their durability and

applicability to current problems, and ask why

some, even to this day, 60 years later, demonstrate their inherently sound foundation.

 A. Jean Ayres had a good idea. This book is

a testament to the durability of Ayres’ initial idea

and the science that followed and is an important

milestone for the editors and authors who have

made contributions to her legacy.

 Reynolds , P. D. ( 1971 ). A primer in theory

construction . Indianapolis, IN : Bobbs Merrill

Educational Publishing .

xi

PREFACE

 More than a decade has passed since we published the second edition of Sensory Integration:

Theory and Practice. A lot has happened in that

time, and, as much as possible, we have tried

to capture the evolution. We have made several

notable changes from the second edition.

 Ayres developed sensory integration (SI)

theory in the 1960s at a time when the occupational therapy profession, struggling to gain

credibility, embraced a medical model. Her

driving force was to explain links between brain

processing and observable behavior. Nonetheless, although participation in everyday activity was traditionally the core of occupational

therapy, Ayres began her work in an era when

many occupational therapists believed that

changing body structure and function would

translate automatically into improved function.

Some therapists who employed SI therapy fell

into that trap. What could be more seductive

than changing the brain? But over-emphasis on

changing the brain sometimes resulted in relegating occupation to the back seat. We now

know that changing the brain matters, but only

if those changes contribute to making everyday

life easier and more meaningful—and that does

not occur automatically. Publication of the International Classifi cation of Functioning Disability

and Health (ICF) has shifted the attention and

beliefs of all health-care professionals toward

activities and participation. In this third edition

of Sensory Integration: Theory and Practice, we

give greater prominence to SI in everyday life.

Among other more subtle changes throughout

the book, we expanded the chapter on SI and

occupation from the second edition and placed it

where it should be—near the front.

 Ayres died in 1989 and she still is sorely

missed. For many years, colleagues who had

worked with her carried the torch that Ayres

lighted. Through time, as might be expected,

some of those colleagues shifted their focus

to specifi c aspects of the theory. Ayres emphasized praxis and dyspraxia. She labeled “tactile

defensiveness” and saw it as a refl ection of poor

sensory modulation. However, she did not live

long enough to fully develop her explanations.

In the ensuing decades, while the theory did not

change fundamentally, the terminology associated

with it morphed. At the same time, many occupational therapy educators relegated SI theory and

therapy to continuing professional education,

perhaps believing it was beyond that required for

basic preparation of entry-level practitioners. All

these factors seemed to conspire to make occupational therapists (and ultimately other professionals) increasingly unclear about the terminology

associated with SI theory and what actually constituted sensory integrative therapy. Throughout

this third edition of Sensory Integration: Theory

and Practice, we have attempted to clarify the

terminology and the principles of intervention.

We also offer a new chapter ( Chapter 3 ) tracing

the history of the development of SI theory–with

all its triumphs and dramas—from Ayres through

to the present. We think that chapter makes a particularly good read.

 Research evidence for both the theory and the

intervention has risen to a higher level. Studies

of SI now include government-funded randomized controlled trials (RCTs) as well as quasiexperimental, correlational, and descriptive

studies that set the stage for additional RCTs

needed to test the effectiveness of this complex

intervention. In this third edition, we devote

two chapters to that evidence. One chapter

( Chapter 15 ) describes the clinically based

research applicable in everyday practice. The

second ( Chapter 16 ) describes a growing body of

basic research underpinning SI theory. We also

have expanded the chapters on praxis and sensory

modulation. Refl ecting the explosion of research

in the area, we also expanded the chapter on

structure and function of the sensory systems

( Chapter 4 ), augmenting it with some information

on interoception, and added a separate chapter on

sensory discrimination (Chapter 7).

 Through time, we became increasingly aware

of the need to illustrate the multiple complex

aspects of assessment related to SI theory. In this

xii ■ Preface

third edition of Sensory Integration: Theory and

Practice, we reintroduced a chapter describing

the Sensory Integration and Praxis Tests (SIPT;

 Chapter 8) ; introduced an entire chapter on clinical observations ( Chapter 9) ; and expanded the

information on assessing without the SIPT into a

chapter on its own ( Chapter 10 ). We retained an

updated and expanded version of the chapter on

interpreting test results related to sensory integrative dysfunction, illustrating that process with

multiple case examples ( Chapter 11 ).

 Since the second edition of Sensory Integration: Theory and Practice, Parham and colleagues

developed and published a Fidelity Measure that

operationally defi nes sensory integrative therapy.

In providing clarifi cation of what actually constitutes sensory integrative therapy, the Fidelity

Measure has, arguably, had the greatest single

effect on the intervention chapters. In this third

edition, we label sensory integrative therapy as a

direct intervention with particular characteristics.

We offer a new chapter ( Chapter 14 ) illustrating

use of the Fidelity Measure as well as Miller and

colleagues’ STEP-SI and A SECRET models

as a means for distilling theory in practice and

making it more readily accessible. We retained

the chapters describing the art and science of

sensory integrative therapy ( Chapters 12 and 13 ,

respectively), but those chapters are so updated

that they barely resemble their second edition

predecessors.

 In this third edition of Sensory Integration:

Theory and Practice, w e clearly separate sensory

integrative therapy from indirect approaches

that often employ SI or related theories (i.e.,

coaching). We offer a new chapter on coaching ( Chapter 17 ) that draws on, but is much

expanded over, the second edition chapter that

described use of SI theory as the basis for consultation in schools.

 Recognizing the growth in thinking and options

regarding occupational therapy intervention for

children with motor coordination or sensory

regulation issues, we offer a new case-based

chapter ( Chapter 22 ) in which we view intervention for a child with poor motor coordination,

fi rst through a sensory integrative lens and then

through a Cognitive Orientation to daily Occupational Performance (CO-OP) lens. We retained

and updated a chapter on sensory-based interventions often used as a complement to or instead

of sensory integrative therapy ( Chapter 18 ),

including the Wilbarger Approach; the Alert

Program; Interactive Metronome®; Astronaut

Training; Infi nity Walk Training; Therapeutic

Listening®; Suck, Swallow, Breathe; and Aquatic

Therapy. In an attempt to be clear about the

relationship of each of these programs to SI, we

analyzed each in terms of three factors: whether

(1) the sensation provided is uni-modal or multimodal; (2) the approach is responsive or prescribed; and (3) the setting in which it is delivered

is traditional, nontraditional, or both. The authors

of each program described the background, rationale, and relationship of the program to SI and

occupation; benefi ts; and populations for which

it is appropriate.

 We have peppered all the chapters in this

third edition with case examples. In addition, we

have included several chapters entirely devoted

to applying theory, assessment, or intervention

principles in a case context. We retained and

expanded a second edition chapter in which we

illustrate the process of planning and implementing intervention for the child with sensory integrative dysfunction ( Chapter 20 ) who was a case

example in the interpretation chapter described

earlier ( Chapter 11 ). We complement that chapter

with one comprising a case example of planning

and implementing intervention for a child with

autism spectrum disorder ( Chapter 21 ).

 The question of whether or not sensory integrative therapy is effective theory has always generated conversation and controversy both within

and outside occupational therapy. In Chapters 15

and 16, we summarize the evidence from basic

science and from clinical studies. We close this

third edition with a chapter ( Chapter 23 ) addressing the complex question of effectiveness explicitly. We remind readers that sensory integrative

therapy comprises both art and science—and that

to try to divorce one from the other is to destroy

the essence of the intervention. Thus, researchers

who have failed to consider both art and science

have failed to test the effectiveness of sensory

integrative therapy. Therefore, they cannot know

whether or not it works.

 We closed the Preface of the second edition

with a comment that seems as apt for this edition

as it was more than a decade ago:

 Sensory integration theory, as much as any

theory in occupational therapy, depends on a

partnership of art and science. Science gives

sensory integration credibility; art gives it

Preface ■ xiii

meaning. Toward a partnership of art and

science, we offer these [new] works from a

number of outstanding theorists, researchers,

clinicians, and artists. Jean Ayres touched us all.

We carry the torch that she passed to us at her

death, fueling it with new perspectives and new

knowledge.

 At the request of several reviewers and to make

this third edition more usable than previous editions for teaching, we added several pedagogical

features. Among these: chapter objectives, periodic “Here ’ s the Point” summaries, refl ections

of Practice Wisdom, brief summaries of relevant

studies we have labeled “Here ’ s the Evidence,”

and “enrichment” reading we have called “Where

Can I Find More?” We hope these, and all the

added features, will make learning and teaching

this content easier and more enjoyable. Above

all, we sought to create a text that students and

practitioners want to read. We hope we have met

our objective.

 Anita C. Bundy

 Shelly J. Lane


xv

CONTRIBUTORS

Teal W. Benevides, PhD, OTR/L

 Department of Occupational Therapy

 Jefferson College of Health Professions

 Philadelphia, Pennsylvania

Rosemarie Bigsby, ScD, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Clinical Professor of Pediatrics, Psychiatry, and

Human Behavior

 The Warren Alpert Medical School of Brown

University

 Coordinator, NICU Services at the Brown

Center for the Study of Children at Risk

 Providence, Rhode Island

Erna Imperatore Blanche, PhD, FAOTA, OTR/L

 Professor of Clinical Occupational Therapy

 USC Chan Division of Occupational Science

and Occupational Therapy

 University of Southern California

 Los Angeles, California

Kim Bulkeley, PhD, BAppSc (OT)

 Faculty of Health Sciences

 University of Sydney

 Sydney, Australia

Anita C. Bundy, ScD, OT/L, FAOTA

 Professor and Department Head, Occupational

Therapy

 Colorado State University

 Ft. Collins, Colorado

Sharon A. Cermak, EdD, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Professor, Joint Appointment with the Keck

School of Medicine of USC

 Department of Pediatrics

 USC Chan Division of Occupational Science

and Occupational Therapy

 Los Angeles, California

Tina Champagne, OTD, OTR/L

 Director of Occupational Therapy

 Cutchins Programs for Children and Families

 Northampton, Massachusetts

Robyn Chu, MOT, OTR/L

 Owner of Growing Healthy Therapy Services

 California, USA

 Faculty STAR Institute for Sensory Processing

Disorder

 Denver, Colorado

Joanna Cosbey, PhD, OTR/L

 Assistant Professor

 Occupational Therapy Graduate Program

 The University of New Mexico

 Albuquerque, New Mexico

Rachel Dumont, MS, OTR/L

 Research Assistant, Department of Occupational

Therapy

 Jefferson College of Health Professions

 Philadelphia, Pennsylvania

Patricia Faller, OTD, OTR/L

 Children ’ s Specialized Hospital

 Toms River, New Jersey

Sheila Frick, OTR/L

 Therapeutic Resources Inc.

 Madison, Wisconsin

Gudrun Gjesing

 Occupational Therapist

 Specialist in Children ’ s Health & Baby

Swimming Instructor

 Halliwick Lecturer

 Haderslev, Denmark

Michael E. Gorman, PhD

 Professor of Engineering and Society

 University of Virginia

 Charlottesville, Virginia

Dido Green, PhD, MSc, DipCOT

 Reader in Rehabilitation

 Oxford Brookes University

 Oxford, United Kingdom

xvi ■ Contributors

Colleen Hacker, MS, OTR

 Sensory Gym

 Hobartville, New South Wales, Australia

Joanne Hunt, OTD, OTR/L

 Children ’ s Specialized Hospital

 Mountainside, New Jersey

Mary Kawar, MS, OT/L

 Mary Kawar & Associates

 San Diego, California

JoAnn Kennedy, OTD, MS, OTR/L

 OT-Family Connections

 Fairfax, Virginia

Dominique Blanche Kiefer, OTD, OTR/L

 Director of Operations

 Therapy West, Inc.

 Los Angeles, California

Shelly J. Lane, PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Professor, Occupational Therapy

 Colorado State University

 Ft. Collins, Colorado

Teresa A. May-Benson, ScD, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Executive Director

 Spiral Foundation

 Newton, Massachusetts

Molly McEwen, MHS, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Consultant

 Hillsboro, Oregon

Lucy J. Miller, PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Founder and Director Emeritus

 Sensory Integration Dysfunction Treatment

and Research Center (STAR)

 Research Director

 Sensory Processing Disorder Foundation

 Denver, Colorado

Shelley Mulligan, PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Associate Professor

 Department of Occupational Therapy

 University of New Hampshire

 Durham, New Hampshire

Patricia Oetter, MA, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Private Consultant

 Therapy Coordinator

 Camp Avanti

 Amery, Wisconsin

Beth T. Osten, MS, OTR/L

 Owner and Director

 Beth Osten & Associates Pediatric Therapy

Services

 Northbrook, Illinois

L. Diane Parham, PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Professor, Occupational Therapy Graduate

Program

 School of Medicine

 University of New Mexico

 Albuquerque, New Mexico

Michele Parkins, MS, OTR

 Founder & Executive Director

 Great Kids Place

 New Jersey, USA

 Faculty STAR Institute for Sensory Processing

Disorder

 Denver, Colorado

Beth Pfeiffer, PhD, OTR/L, BCP

 Associate Professor, Department

of Rehabilitation Sciences

 Temple University

 Philadelphia, Pennsylvania

Gustavo Reinoso, PhD, OTR/L

 Assistant Professor, Department of Occupational

Therapy

 Nova Southeastern University-Tampa

 Director, Advanced Therapy Systems (ATS)

 Dundalk, Co Louth, Ireland

Stacey Reynolds, PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Associate Professor

 Virginia Commonwealth University

 Richmond, Virginia

Eileen W. Richter, MPH, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Retired Former Co-Director, Camp Avanti

 Amery, Wisconsin

 Richter Active Integration Resources http://

www.richterair.com/

Contributors ■ xvii

Roseann C. Schaaf, PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Department of Occupational Therapy, Jefferson

College of Health Professions

 Faculty of the Farber Institute of Neuroscience,

Thomas Jefferson University

 Philadelphia, Pennsylvania

Sarah A. Schoen, PhD, OTR

 Associate Director of Research

 Sensory Processing Disorder Foundation

 Associate Professor

 Rocky Mountain University of Health

Professions

 Denver, Colorado

Sherry Shellenberger, OTR/L

 Co-Owner, TherapyWorks Inc.

 Albuquerque, New Mexico

Susanne Smith Roley, OTD, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Occupational Therapist

 CenterPoint for Children

 Founding Partner, Collaborative for Leadership

in Ayres Sensory Integration (CLASI)

 Irvine, California

Virginia Spielmann, MS OT, PhD (cand)

 Executive Director

 STAR Institute for Sensory Processing Disorder

 Denver, Colorado

Stacey Szklut, MS, OTR/L

 Executive Director

 South Shore Therapies Inc.

 Weymouth, Massachusetts

Elke van Hooydonk, OTD, OTR/L

 Children ’ s Specialized Hospital

 Toms River, New Jersey

Julia Wilbarger, PhD, OTR/L

 Chair, Associate Professor, Occupational

Therapy

 Dominican University of California

 San Rafael, California

Patricia Wilbarger, MEd, OTR/L

 Retired

 Santa Barbara, California

MarySue Williams, OTR/L

 Co-Owner, TherapyWorks Inc.

 Albuquerque, New Mexico

xxiii

CONTENTS IN BRIEF

 Foreword ix

Preface xi

Contributors xv

Reviewers xix

PART I Theoretical Constructs

1 Sensory Integration: A. Jean Ayres’ Theory Revisited 2

 2 Sensory Integration in Everyday Life 21

 3 Composing a Theory: An Historical Perspective 40

PART II The Neuroscience Basis of Sensory Integration Disorders

4 Structure and Function of the Sensory Systems 58

 5 Praxis and Dyspraxia 115

 6 Sensory Modulation Functions and Disorders 151

 7 Sensory Discrimination Functions and Disorders 181

PART III Tools for Assessment

8 Assessment of Sensory Integration Functions Using the Sensory Integration

and Praxis Tests 208

 9 Using Clinical Observations within the Evaluation Process 222

 10 Assessing Sensory Integrative Dysfunction without the SIPT 243

 11 Interpreting and Explaining Evaluation Data 256

PART IV Intervention

12 The Art of Therapy 286

 13 The Science of Intervention: Creating Direct Intervention from Theory 300

 14 Distilling Sensory Integration Theory for Use: Making Sense of the Complexity 338

PART V Complementing and Extending Theory and Application

15 Advances in Sensory Integration Research: Clinically Based Research 352

 16 Advances in Sensory Integration Research: Basic Science Research 371

 17 Using Sensory Integration Theory in Coaching 393

 18 Complementary Programs for Intervention 423

 19 Application of Sensory Integration with Specifi c Populations 479

PART VI Cases

20 Planning and Implementing Intervention Using Sensory Integration Theory 532

 21 Planning and Implementing Intervention: A Case Example of a Child with Autism 548

 22 Viewing Intervention Through Different Lenses 560

 23 Is Sensory Integration Effective? A Complicated Question to End the Book 568

xix

REVIEWERS

Auriela Alexander, OTD, OTR/L

 Associate Professor

 Division of Occupational Therapy

 Florida A&M University

 Tallahassee, Florida

Cindy Anderson, OTD, OTR/L

 Associate Professor, Occupational Therapy

 University of Mary

 Bismarck, North Dakota

Evelyn Anderson, PhD, OTR/L

 Associate Professor

 Occupational Therapy Program

 Midwestern University

 Downers Grove, Illinois

Amy Armstrong-Heimsoth, OTD, OTR/L

 Assistant Clinical Professor, Occupational

Therapy

 Northern Arizona University

 Phoenix, Arizona

Tamara Avi-Itzhak, D.Sc.

 Associate Professor, Occupational Therapy

 York College, The City University of New York

 Jamaica, New York

Stephanie Beisbier, OTD, OTR/L

 Assistant Professor, Professional Entry Program

Director

 Occupational Therapy

 Mount Mary University

 Milwaukee, Wisconsin

Erna Imperatore Blanche, PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Associate Professor of Clinical Practice

 Chan Division of Occupational Science

and Occupational Therapy at Ostrow

 School of Dentistry

 University of Southern California

 Los Angeles, California

Jason Browning, MOT, OTR/L

 Assistant Professor of Occupational Therapy

 Jefferson College of Health Sciences

 Roanoke, Virginia

Kim Bryze, PhD, OTR/L

 Program Director and Professor, Occupational

Therapy Program

 College of Health Sciences

 Midwestern University

 Downers Grove, Illinois

Debra Collette Allen, OTD, OTR/L

 Assistant Professor, Occupational Therapy

 The Sage Colleges

 Troy, New York

Lisa Crabtree, PhD, OTR/L

 Assistant Professor, Graduate Faculty

 Occupational Therapy and Occupational Science

 Towson University

 Towson, Maryland

Denise Donica, DHSc, OTR/L, BCP, FAOTA

 Associate Professor, Occupational Therapy

 East Carolina University

 Greenville, North Carolina

Deborah Dougherty, OTD, MS, OTR

 Program Director, Graduate Occupational

Therapy Program

 Department of Health Professions

 Mercy College

 Dobbs Ferry, New York

Beth Elenko, PhD, OTR/L

 Assistant Professor, Occupational Therapy

 SUNY Downstate Medical Center

 Brooklyn, New York

Nancy Gabres, MS, OTR/L

 Assistant Professor, Occupational Therapy

 The College of St. Scholastica

 Duluth, Minnesota

xx ■ Reviewers

Elizabeth Hebert, PhD, OTR/L

 Assistant Professor of Occupational Therapy

 Nazareth College

 Rochester, New York

Caroline Hills, DipCOT, BSc, MSc, PCTE, PhD

 Practice Education Coordinator, Occupational

Therapy

 School of Health Sciences

 National University of Ireland Galway (NUIG)

 Galway, Ireland

Gregory Patrick Kelly, PhD, BSc (Hons)

 Reader in Teaching and Learning, Senior Fellow

of the Higher Education Academy

 School of Health Sciences

 Ulster University

 Newtownabbey, Co. Antrim, Northern Ireland

Mary Khetani, ScD, OTR/L

 Assistant Professor, Occupational Therapy

 University of Illinois at Chicago

 Chicago, Illinois

Heather Kuhaneck, PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Associate Professor, Occupational Therapy

 Sacred Heart University

 Fairfi eld, Connecticut

Fengyi Kuo, DHS, OTR, CPRP

 Visiting Professor; Occupational Therapy

Consultant

 Occupational Therapy; Training and

Professional Development

 Indiana University; LIH Olivia ’ s Place

 Beijing & Shanghai, China

Alicia Lutman, OTD, MS, OTR/L, ATC

 Associate Professor, Occupational Therapy

 Shenandoah University

 Winchester, Virginia

Heather Martin, MS, OTR

 Instructor, Occupational Therapy

 Mount Mary University

 Milwaukee, Wisconsin

Ellen McLaughlin, EdD, OTR/L

 Associate Professor, Occupational Therapy

 Misericordia University

 Dallas, Pennsylvania

Constance C. Messier, OTR/L, OTD

 Assistant Professor, Occupational Therapy

 Department of Occupational Therapy

 Salem State University

 Salem, Massachusetts

Michelle Mounteney, MS/OTR/L

 Occupational Therapist and Clinical Assistant

Professor

 Occupational Therapy

 D’Youville College

 Buffalo, New York

Shirley P. O’Brien, PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Professor/Foundation Professor, Occupational

Science and Occupational Therapy

 Eastern Kentucky University

 Richmond, Kentucky

Laurette Olson, PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Professor, Graduate Program in Occupational

Therapy

 School of Health and Natural Sciences

 Mercy College

 Dobbs Ferry, New York

Rena Purohit, JD, OTR/L

 Assistant Professor, Occupational Therapy

 Touro College

 New York, New York

Ellen Berger Rainville, OTD, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Professor, Occupational Therapy

 Springfi eld College

 Springfi eld, Massachusetts

Teresa Schlabach, PhD, OTR/L, BCP

 Associate Dean, College of Health and Human

Services

 Professor, Occupational Therapy

 St. Ambrose University

 Davenport, Iowa

Leann M. Shore, OTD, MEd, OTR/L

 Assistant Professor, Program in Occupational

Therapy

 University of Minnesota

 Minneapolis, Minnesota

Reviewers ■ xxi

Patricia Steffen-Sanchez, MS, OTR/L

 Assistant Professor, Occupational Therapy

 Midwestern University, College of Health

Sciences

 Glendale, Arizona

Pamela Stephenson, OTD, MS, OTR/L

 Assistant Professor of Occupational Therapy

 Murphy Deming College of Health Sciences

(Mary Baldwin University)

 Staunton, Virginia

MaryEllen Thompson, PhD, OTR/L

 Professor and Graduate Coordinator

 Occupational Science and Occupational Therapy

 Eastern Kentucky University

 Richmond, Kentucky

Ingris Treminio, DrOT, OTR/L

 Clinical Assistant Professor

 Department of Occupational Therapy

 Florida International University

 Miami, Florida


xxv

 CONTENTS

 Foreword ix

Preface xi

Contributors xv

Reviewers xix

PART I Theoretical Constructs

1 Sensory Integration: A. Jean Ayres’ Theory Revisited 2

Anita C. Bundy, ScD, OT/L, FAOTA and Shelly J. Lane, PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Learning Outcomes 2

 Purpose and Scope 2

 Sensory Integrative Dysfunction: Illustrating the Reasoning 3

 CASE: Joshua 3

 Introduction to Sensory Integration Theory 4

Postulates of Sensory Integration Theory 4

 Illustrating Sensory Integration Theory 5

Sensory Integration Theory and Learning 5

Sensory Integrative Dysfunction 6

 The Constructs 9

Dyspraxia 10

Sensory Modulation Dysfunction 11

 Uniting Sensory Integration with Psychosocial Constructs 12

 CASE: Joe 12

The Spiral Process of Self-Actualization 13

 All Theories Are Based on Underlying Assumptions 15

 Boundaries of Sensory Integration Theory and Intervention 15

Boundaries and the Population 15

Boundaries and Intervention 17

Boundaries and Critique 17

 Summary and Conclusions 17

 Where Can I Find More? 17

 References 18

 2 Sensory Integration in Everyday Life 21

L. Diane Parham, PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA and Joanna Cosbey, PhD, OTR/L

 Learning Outcomes 21

 Purpose and Scope 21

 CASE: Nick 22

 The Complexity of Everyday Life 23

 Sensory Integration and Everyday Life: The Evidence 24

Play, Leisure, and Social Participation 25

Activities of Daily Living and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living 29

Rest and Sleep 30

Education and Work 30

xxvi ■ Contents

 Implications for Assessment and Intervention 31

Assessment: Looking to the Future, Considering the Past 32

Consideration of Intervention Options 34

 Summary and Conclusions 35

 Where Can I Find More? 36

 References 36

 3 Composing a Theory: An Historical Perspective 40

Shelly J. Lane, PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA, Anita C. Bundy, ScD, OT/L, FAOTA, and Michael E. Gorman, PhD

 Learning Outcomes 40

 Purpose and Scope 40

 A Little Background 40

Ayres the Person 41

Ayres the Professional: Developing Her Knowledge Base 42

 Growth of Sensory Integration Theory and Research 45

Research and the Center for the Study of Sensory Integrative Dysfunction 45

SII and Growing Tension 46

 Evolution of Ayres’ Work 49

Moving Forward 51

 Summary and Conclusions 52

 Acknowledgments 53

 Where Can I Find More? 53

 References 54

PART II The Neuroscience Basis of Sensory Integration Disorders

 4 Structure and Function of the Sensory Systems 58

Shelly J. Lane, PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Learning Outcomes 58

 Purpose and Scope 58

 Basic Structure and Function of the Central Nervous System 58

Cells of the Central Nervous System 59

 Central and Peripheral Nervous System Structure 60

Central Nervous System Geography 63

Central Nervous System Function 64

Terminology 65

 The Somatosensory System 69

Receptors and Transduction 69

Dorsal Column Medial Lemniscal (DCML) Pathway 73

Interpreting Somatosensory Input 74

Spinocerebellar Pathways 77

Anterolateral (AL) System 78

Somatosensation from the Face 80

Functional Considerations 80

 Interoception 82

Receptors and Transduction 82

Interpreting Interoceptive Input 83

Functional Considerations 83

 The Vestibular System 84

Receptors and Transduction 84

Central Projections 88

Contents ■ xxvii

The Integrative Vestibular System 91

Vestibular and Proprioception Interactions 91

 The Auditory System 91

Receptors and Transduction 92

Central Connections 93

Efferent Processes and Feedback Loops 95

 The Visual System 95

Receptors and Transduction 96

Central Connections 98

Visual Experience Counts 100

 Gustation and Olfaction 100

Taste and Taste Receptors 100

Taste Pathways 101

Smell and Smell Receptors 102

Smell Pathways 103

Clinical Links to Taste or Smell Sensitivity Differences 104

 Summary and Conclusions 106

 Where Can I Find More? 109

 References 109

 5 Praxis and Dyspraxia 115

Sharon A. Cermak, EdD, OTR/L, FAOTA and Teresa A. May-Benson, ScD, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Learning Outcomes 115

 Introduction 115

 Purpose and Scope 116

 The Role of Sensation in Movement and Praxis 116

Tactile System 117

Proprioception 117

Vestibular System 118

Vision 118

Auditory Processing 119

 Assessing Disorders of Sensory Integration and Praxis 119

 CASE: Alyssa 120

Reason for Referral 120

Parent Interview and Developmental/Sensory History 120

Teacher Questionnaire 121

 CASE: Dalton 123

Reason for Referral 123

Parent Interview and Developmental/Sensory History 123

Teacher Interview 123

 Disorders of Praxis 124

Patterns of Practic Dysfunction 124

 Neuroanatomical Bases of Praxis 128

Ideation 128

Planning, Motor Learning, and Execution 129

Neuroimaging Findings in Children with Dyspraxia or DCD 131

 Related Diagnoses and Terminology 131

Related Diagnoses 131

 Dyspraxia Across Ages 133

Early Childhood 133

School Years 134

Adolescence and Adulthood 135

xxviii ■ Contents

Behavioral and Social-Emotional Characteristics of Children with Dyspraxia 135

Cognitive and Executive Function 136

 The Intervention Process 137

Sensory Integration Principles for Praxis Intervention 137

Interventions for Motor Planning and Motor Coordination 138

Intervention for Ideation 140

 CASE: Intervention for Alyssa and Dalton 141

 Evidence for Interventions for Dyspraxia 142

 Summary and Conclusions 143

 Where Can I Find More? 143

 References 143

 6 Sensory Modulation Functions and Disorders 151

Shelly J. Lane, PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Learning Outcomes 151

 Purpose and Scope 151

 CASE: Michael 152

 Sensory Modulation 152

Modulation as a Physiological Process at the Cellular Level 152

Modulation at the Level of Systems and Behavior 155

 Sensory Modulation Dysfunction 157

A Brief Historical Overview 157

Proposed Central Nervous System Links to Sensory Modulation Dysfunction 159

 Sensory Modulation Disorders 167

Tactile Defensiveness 167

Aversive Responses to Vestibular and Proprioceptive Inputs, Gravitational Insecurity,

and Vestibular and Proprioceptive Under-Responsiveness 171

Sensory Modulation Dysfunction in Other Sensory Systems 172

Sensory Modulation Disorder in Children with Additional Diagnoses 173

 Summary and Conclusions 175

 Where Can I Find More? 175

 References 176

 7 Sensory Discrimination Functions and Disorders 181

Shelly J. Lane, PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA and Stacey Reynolds, PhD, OTR/L

 Learning Outcomes 181

 Purpose and Scope 181

 Sensory Discrimination 181

 Sensory Discrimination: An Illustration 183

 CASE: Ricky 183

Touch Discrimination 183

Foundations of Somatosensory Discrimination 183

Measurement of Somatosensory Discrimination 185

 Movement Discrimination 186

Foundations of Proprioceptive Discrimination 186

Foundations of Vestibular Discrimination 187

Measurement of Movement Discrimination 187

 Auditory Discrimination 190

Discrimination of “What” We Hear 190

Discrimination of “Where” We Hear 191

Measurement of Auditory Discrimination 192

Contents ■ xxix

 Visual Discrimination 193

Foundations of Visual Perception and Discrimination 193

Measurement of Visual Perception and Discrimination 197

 Taste and Smell Discrimination 199

Foundations of Taste Discrimination 199

Foundations of Smell Discrimination 200

Measurement of Taster Status and Taste or Smell Discrimination 200

 Summary and Conclusions 201

 Where Can I Find More? 201

 References 201

PART III Tools for Assessment

 8 Assessment of Sensory Integration Functions Using the Sensory Integration

and Praxis Tests 208

Shelley Mulligan, PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Learning Outcomes 208

 Purpose and Scope 208

 Description and Purpose of the Sensory Integration and Praxis Tests 208

 Validity and Reliability of the Sensory Integration and Praxis Tests 211

Validity 211

Reliability 213

 Analyses of SIPT Scores with Other Assessment Data for Completing Comprehensive

Evaluations of Children 213

Synthesis of Evaluation Data 214

 CASE: Using the SIPT in the Evaluation Process: Lilly 216

 Summary and Conclusions 220

 Where Can I Find More? 220

 References 220

 9 Using Clinical Observations within the Evaluation Process 222

Erna Imperatore Blanche, PhD, FAOTA, OTR/L, Gustavo Reinoso, PhD, OTR/L, and

Dominique Blanche Kiefer, OTD, OTR/L

 Learning Outcomes 222

 Purpose and Scope 222

 Assessment and Interpretation 226

Postural-Ocular Control 226

Prone Extension 226

Motor Planning 231

Additional Observations of Sensory Processing 236

 Interpretation of Results 239

 Summary and Conclusions 240

 Where Can I Find More? 240

 References 241

 10 Assessing Sensory Integrative Dysfunction without the SIPT 243

Anita C. Bundy, ScD, OT/L, FAOTA

 Learning Outcomes 243

 Purpose and Scope 243

 Introduction 243

xxx ■ Contents

 Sensory Integration Theory Revisited 244

 CASE: Lenard 245

Dyspraxia 247

 CASE: Looking at Lenard ’ s Praxis 249

 Assessment of Somatosensory Discrimination 249

 Assessment of Postural and Ocular Control 250

 CASE: Lenard ’ s Clinical Observations Performance 251

 A Need for Caution and Clinical Reasoning in Testing without the SIPT 251

 Assessment of Sensory Modulation Disorders 252

Sensory Processing Measure (SPM) 252

Sensory Profi le-2 (SP2) 252

 CASE: Lenard ’ s Sensory Modulation and Our Conclusions 253

 Summary and Conclusions 254

 Where Can I Find More? 254

 References 254

 11 Interpreting and Explaining Evaluation Data 256

Anita C. Bundy, ScD, OT/L, FAOTA, Susanne Smith Roley, OTD, OTR/L, FAOTA, Zoe Mailloux, OTD, OTR/L,

FAOTA, L. Diane Parham, PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA, and Shelly J. Lane, PhD, OTR, FAOTA

 Learning Outcomes 256

 Purpose and Scope 256

 Introduction 256

 Referral and Developmental History 257

 CASE: Kyle 257

 Research Related to Sensory Integration and Praxis Patterns 261

 CASE: Kyle: Interpreting the Results 272

Meaningful Clusters 272

Using the Interpretation Worksheet 272

The Final Stage of Interpretation 276

Reporting the Results 277

 Somatodyspraxia 278

 CASE: Jackie 278

 Summary and Conclusions 281

 Where Can I Find More? 281

 References 282

PART IV Intervention

 12 The Art of Therapy 286

Anita C. Bundy, ScD, OT/L, FAOTA and Colleen Hacker, MS, OTR

 Learning Outcomes 286

 Introduction 286

 Purpose and Scope 287

 CASE: Phoebe 287

 The Artful Therapist: A Good Playmate 290

Vision 291

Auditory 291

Tactile 291

Proprioception 292

Vestibular 292

 Play as the Basis of Sensory Integrative Therapy 292

Defi ning Play 293

Contents ■ xxxi

Play Element 1: Relative Intrinsic Motivation 293

Play Element 2: Relative Internal Control 295

Play Element 3: Freedom from Some Constraints of Reality 296

Play Element 4: Framing 296

 Play and Fidelity to Treatment 297

 Summary and Conclusions 298

 Where Can I Find More? 298

 References 298

 13 The Science of Intervention: Creating Direct Intervention from Theory 300

Anita C. Bundy, ScD, OT/L, FAOTA, FOTARA and Stacey Szklut, MS, OTR/L

 Learning Outcomes 300

 Purpose and Scope 300

 CASE: Sam 302

 Providing Opportunities for Enhanced Sensation 302

Qualities Affecting the Intensity of Sensation 303

 Intervention for Sensory Modulation Dysfunction 304

Treatment Guidelines for Sensory Over-Responsivity 304

Treatment Guidelines for Sensory Under-Responsivity 309

Modulating Arousal 310

 Intervention for Practic Disorders 311

Promoting Planning 311

Promoting Bilateral Integration 315

Promoting Ideation 316

 CASE: Alex 319

Initiating, Carrying Out, and Generalizing New Motor Tasks 319

 Intervention for Increased Sensory Discrimination 320

Vestibular-Proprioceptive Discrimination: Postural-Ocular Control 320

Targeting Other Aspects of Proprioceptive-Vestibular Discrimination 329

 Balancing Intervention for Multiple Types of Sensory Integrative Dysfunction 331

 Practical Considerations for Intervention 331

Parent Involvement 332

Therapist Training 332

Therapist-to-Client Ratio 332

Length of Sessions 332

Physical Environment 332

 Summary and Conclusions 333

 Where Can I Find More? 333

 References 334

 14 Distilling Sensory Integration Theory for Use: Making Sense of the

Complexity 338

 Learning Outcomes 338

 Purpose and Scope 338

 Resources to Guide Direct Intervention 339

Schematic Representation of Sensory Integration Theory 339

 Ayres Sensory Integration ® Fidelity Measure (ASIFM) 339

L. Diane Parham, PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA

 The STEP-SI 344

Lucy J. Miller, PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA

General Principles of STEP-SI 344

 Models to Help Families Thrive 346

xxxii ■ Contents

 A SECRET 346

Lucy J. Miller, PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Summary and Conclusions 348

 Where Can I Find More? 348

 References 348

PART V Complementing and Extending Theory and Application

 15 Advances in Sensory Integration Research: Clinically Based Research 352

Sarah A. Schoen, PhD, OTR, Shelly J. Lane, PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA, and Lucy J. Miller, PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Learning Outcomes 352

 Introduction 352

 Purpose and Scope 353

 Identifying and Defi ning the Disorders; Research Related to Assessment 353

Rating Scales: Standardized Report Measures 353

Standardized Performance Measures 355

Additional Measures of Performance and Parent or Self-Report 356

 Research Related to Intervention 357

Previous Studies of Occupational Therapy with a Sensory-Based Approach 358

Future Directions 361

 Research Related to the Disorders 362

Research Related to Impairments in Sensory Modulation and Sensory Integration 363

Research Related to Prevalence, Risk Factors, and Clinical Presentation 364

Future Directions 366

 Summary and Conclusions 366

 Where Can I Find More? 366

 References 366

 16 Advances in Sensory Integration Research: Basic Science Research 371

Sarah A. Schoen, PhD, OTR, Shelly J. Lane, PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA, Lucy J. Miller, PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA, and

Stacey Reynolds, PhD, OTR/L

 Learning Outcomes 371

 Introduction 371

 Purpose and Scope 373

 Research Related to Underlying Neurological Mechanisms 373

Studies of the Autonomic Nervous System 373

Neuroimaging 375

 Animal Research: From Cages to Clinics 378

Life Span Studies 378

Impact of Treatment 379

Environmental Infl uences and Epigenetic Mechanisms 381

Drawing from Animal Research 383

 Studies of SMD in Populations Comorbid for Other Conditions 383

Sources of Evidence: Physiological and Behavioral 384

 Summary and Conclusions 387

 Where Can I Find More? 387

 References 387

 17 Using Sensory Integration Theory in Coaching 393

Anita C. Bundy, ScD, OT/L, FAOTA and Kim Bulkeley, PhD, BAppSc (OT)

 Learning Outcomes 393

 Purpose and Scope 393

Contents ■ xxxiii

 Myths Surrounding Coaching 394

Myth #1: Coaching Involves Therapists Training Teachers or Parents to Implement

Therapy (i.e., Do the Job of the Therapist) 394

Myth #2: Because a Parent or Teacher Implements the Intervention, Therapists

Spend Less Time with Children and, Therefore, Can Dramatically Increase Their

Caseloads 395

Myth #3: Coaching Is a Substitute for Direct Intervention 395

 Defi ning Practices for Implementing Coaching 395

 Building the Partnership and Need for Resources 398

Building the Partnership 398

Attaining Needed Resources 399

 Examples of Coaching 400

 CASE: Rebecca 400

 CASE: Shaw 401

 CASE: Duncan 401

 Research Evidence for Coaching- and Sensory-Based Interventions Used Commonly in

Coaching with Families of Young Children with Autism 404

Mutual Information Sharing and Support (Category 1) 405

Adapting Tasks or the Environment (Category 2) 408

Embedding Sensory Input into Everyday Activity to Modulate Arousal (Category 3) 408

Self-Regulatory Strategies (Category 4) 410

Universal Design (Category 5) 410

 Summary and Conclusions 411

 Where Can I Find More? 412

 References 412

 18 Complementary Programs for Intervention 423

 Learning Outcomes 423

 Introduction 423

Three Areas of Sensory Integration 424

 Purpose and Scope 424

 Section 1: The Wilbarger Approach to Treating Sensory Defensiveness 426

Julia Wilbarger, PhD, OTR/L and Patricia Wilbarger, MED, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Background 426

 Rationale 426

 Program Description 427

Education 427

Sensory Diet 428

Professionally Guided Intervention 428

 Relationship to Sensory Integration and Occupation 429

 Expected Benefi ts 430

 Target Populations 430

 Training Recommended or Required 431

 CASE: Danielle 431

 Section 2: The Alert Program ® for Self-Regulation 432

MarySue Williams, OTR/L, Sherry Shellenberger, OTR/L, and Molly McEwen, MHS, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Background 432

 Rationale 433

 Program Description 434

 Relationship to Sensory Integration and Occupation 435

 Expected Benefi ts 436

xxxiv ■ Contents

 Target Populations 437

 Training Recommended or Required 437

 CASE: Alert Program ® in a Public School System 437

 Section 3: Aquatic Therapy 439

Gudrun Gjesing, Occupational Therapist, Specialist in Children ’ s Health & Swimming, Coach and Lecturer

 Background 439

 Rationale 439

 Program Description 440

 Relationship to Sensory Integration and Occupation 442

 Expected Benefi ts 442

 Target Populations 442

 Training Recommended or Required 443

 CASE: “The Alarm Clock” 443

 Section 4: Interactive Metronome ® 445

Beth Osten, MS, OTR/L

 Background 445

 Rationale 446

 Program Description 447

 Relationship to Sensory Integration and Occupation 448

 Expected Benefi ts 448

 Target Populations 448

 Training Recommended or Required 449

 Case Examples 449

 CASE: Lars 449

 CASE: George 450

 CASE: Martin 450

 Section 5: Astronaut Training Program 452

Mary Kawar, MS, OT/L

 Background 452

 Rationale 453

 Program Description 453

 Relationship to Sensory Integration and Occupation 455

 Expected Benefi ts 455

 Target Populations 456

 Training Recommended or Required 456

 Case Examples 456

 CASE: Rita 456

 CASE: Robbie 457

 CASE: George 457

 CASE: Page 457

 Section 6: Infi nity Walk Training 458

Mary Kawar, MS, OT/L

 Background 458

 Rationale 458

 Program Description 458

 Relationship to Sensory Integration and Occupation 460

 Expected Benefi ts 460

 Target Populations 460

 Training Recommended or Required 461

 CASE: Kevin 461

Contents ■ xxxv

 Section 7: Therapeutic Listening® 462

Sheila Frick, OTR/L

 Background 462

 Rationale 462

 Program Description 463

 Relationship to Sensory Integration and Occupation 463

 Expected Benefi ts 464

 Target Populations 465

 Training Recommended or Required 465

 CASE: Christopher 465

 Section 8: Applying Suck/Swallow/Breathe Synchrony Strategies to Sensory

Integration Therapy 466

Patricia Oetter, MA, OTR/L, FAOTA and Eileen W. Richter, MOH, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Background 466

 Rationale 467

 Program Description 468

 Relationship to Sensory Integration and Occupation 469

 Expected Benefi ts 470

 Target Populations 470

 Training Recommended or Required 470

 CASE: Elisha 470

 Summary and Conclusions 472

 Where Can I Find More? 473

 References 473

The Wilbarger Approach to Treating Sensory Defensiveness 473

The Alert Program ® for Self-Regulation 474

Aquatic Therapy 475

Interactive Metronome® 475

Astronaut Training 476

Infi nity Walk 477

Therapeutic Listening® 477

Suck, Swallow, Breathe 477

 19 Application of Sensory Integration with Specifi c Populations 479

 Learning Outcomes 479

 Introduction 479

 Section 1: Sensory Integration Applications with Infants in Neonatal Intensive Care

and Early Intervention 481

Rosemarie Bigsby, ScD, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Background and Rationale for Applying Sensory Integration 481

 Sensory Integration in Early Infancy and Associated Occupation-Based

Challenges 481

 Evaluation and Intervention in the NICU 483

 Evaluation and Intervention in Early Intervention Programs 485

 CASE: Lily 487

 Where Can I Find More? 489

 Section 2: Sensory Integration Approaches with Individuals with Attention

Defi cit-Hyperactivity Disorder 489

Shelley Mulligan, PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Background and Rationale for Applying Sensory Integration 489

 Sensory Integration and Associated Occupation-Based Challenges 490

xxxvi ■ Contents

 Evaluation and Intervention 491

 CASE: Morgan 494

 Where Can I Find More? 496

 Section 3: Applying Sensory Integration Principles for Children with Autism

Spectrum Disorder 496

Teal W. Benevides, PhD, OTR/L, Rachel Dumont, OTR/L, MS, and Roseann C. Schaaf, PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Background and Rationale for Applying Sensory Integration 496

 Sensory Integration and Occupation-Based Challenges 496

 Evaluation and Intervention 498

 CASE: Martin 499

 Where Can I Find More? 502

 Section 4: Sensory Integration and Children with Disorders of Trauma and

Attachment 502

JoAnn Kennedy, OTD, MS, OTR/L

 Background and Rationale for Applying Sensory Integration 502

 Sensory Integration and Associated Occupation-Based Challenges 504

 Evaluation and Intervention 504

 CASE: Ted 505

 Where Can I Find More? 507

 Section 5: Sensory Integration Applications with Adults 507

Beth Pfeiffer, PhD, OTR/L, BCP

 Background and Rationale for Applying Sensory Integration 507

 Sensory Integration and Associated Occupation-Based Challenges 508

 Evaluation and Intervention 508

 CASE: George 511

 Where Can I Find More? 512

 Section 6: Sensory Integration Approaches with Adults with Mental Health

Disorders 513

Tina Champagne, OTD, OTR/L and Beth Pfeiffer, PhD, OTR/L, BCP

 Background and Rationale for Applying Sensory Integration and the Impact on

Occupation 513

Schizophrenia 513

Anxiety Disorders 514

Trauma and Stress-Related Disorders 515

Mood Disorders 516

 Evaluation and Intervention 516

 Case Studies 518

 CASE: Janelle 518

 CASE: Amy 519

 Where Can I Find More? 521

 Summary and Conclusions 521

 References 521

PART VI Cases

 20 Planning and Implementing Intervention Using Sensory Integration

Theory 532

Anita C. Bundy, ScD, OT/L, FAOTA and Susanne Smith Roley, OTD, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Learning Outcomes 532

 Purpose and Scope 532

Contents ■ xxxvii

 Introduction 532

 Kyle Revisited 533

Conducting the Comprehensive Evaluation 533

Generating Hypotheses 533

Developing and Setting Goals and Objectives 534

Summary of the Intervention Plan 537

Setting the Stage for Intervention 537

Providing Intervention 540

 Summary and Conclusions 546

 Where Can I Find More? 547

 References 547

 21 Planning and Implementing Intervention: A Case Example of a Child

with Autism 548

Roseann C. Schaaf, PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA, Joanne Hunt, OTD, OTR/L, Elke van Hooydonk, OTD, OTR/L,

Patricia Faller, OTD, OTR/L, Teal W. Benevides, PhD, OTR/L, and Rachel Dumont, OTR/L, MS

 Learning Outcomes 548

 Purpose and Scope 548

 Introduction 548

 Kendra Revisited 549

Identifying Participation Challenges 549

Conducting the Comprehensive Evaluation 550

Generating Hypotheses 551

Developing and Setting Goals and Objectives 552

Context and Schedule for Service Delivery 553

Setting the Stage for Intervention 554

Providing Intervention 554

Ongoing Clinical Reasoning 557

Outcomes Following 10 Weeks of Intervention 557

 Summary and Conclusions 558

 Where Can I Find More? 558

 References 558

 22 Viewing Intervention Through Different Lenses 560

Anita C. Bundy, ScD, OT/L, FAOTA and Dido Green, PhD, MSc, DipCOT

 Learning Outcomes 560

 Purpose and Scope 560

 Looking at Lars Through a Modifi ed Sensory Integrative Lens 561

Discussion 562

 The CO-OP Approach™ 564

Looking at Lars Through the CO-OP Approach™ Lens 564

Second Goal: Bike Riding 566

 Summary and Conclusions 567

 Where Can I Find More? 567

 References 567

 23 Is Sensory Integration Effective? A Complicated Question to

End the Book 568

Anita C. Bundy, ScD, OT/L, FAOTA and Shelly J. Lane, PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Learning Outcomes 568

 Purpose and Scope 568

 Sensory Integration as Science 569

 The Art of Therapy 570

xxxviii ■ Contents

 The Challenge of Finding Effectiveness 572

 Sensory Integration as Part of Occupational Therapy 574

 Summary and Conclusions 575

 Where Can I Find More? 575

 References 575

Appendix The STAR Process: An Overview 578

Lucy J. Miller, PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA, Robyn C. Chu, MOT, OTR/L, Michele Parkins, MS, OTR,

Virginia Spielmann, MSOT, and Sarah A. Schoen, PhD, OTR

 Glossary 585

 Index 595

PART

I

Theoretical

Constructs

2

CHAPTER

1

Sensory Integration:

A. Jean Ayres’ Theory Revisited

 Anita C. Bundy , ScD, OT/L, FAOTA ■ Shelly J. Lane , PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Chapter 1

Upon completion of this chapter, the reader will be able to:

✔ Describe the basic principles of sensory

integration (SI) theory.

✔ Compare and contrast the various schematic

representations of SI theory, realizing that they

all illustrate the same theory.

✔ Describe a hypothesized relationship between

constructs associated with SI theory and the

Model of Human Occupation (e.g., volition;

belief in skills).

✔ Compare and contrast two main categories

of SI dysfunction: dyspraxia and sensory

modulation dysfunction.

✔ Distinguish between two types of dyspraxia

(i.e., defi cits in bilateral integration and

sequencing, somatodyspraxia) and two types

of sensory modulation dysfunction (i.e.,

over-responsivity; under-responsivity) in terms

of hypothesized sensory bases and overt

indicators.

✔ Identify assumptions and boundaries of SI

theory and SI intervention.

 Just as the continued production of research results in constantly changing

neurological concepts, so also will [sensory integration]

theory need to undergo frequent revision.

 —A. Jean Ayres ( 1972b, p . ix)

LEARNING OUTCOMES

Purpose and Scope

 A. Jean Ayres’ sensory integration (SI) theory

has sparked more research, had a more marked

effect on, and generated more controversy than

any other theory developed by an occupational

therapist. Originally, Ayres, an occupational therapist with advanced training in neuroscience and

educational psychology, developed SI theory to

explain relationships between defi cits in interpreting sensation from the body and the environment and diffi culties with academic and motor

learning.

 Ayres’ knowledge of neuroscience led her to

observe defi cits in learning and behavior and

hypothesize that those defi cits were the result

of poor processing of sensation in the central

nervous system (CNS). In other words, she

hypothesized relationships among areas of the

CNS and various behavioral constructs. Although

knowledge of neurophysiology has grown exponentially in the intervening years, many of the

links that Ayres hypothesized have been upheld.

(See Chapter 4 , Structure and Function of the

Sensory Systems, and Chapter 6 , Sensory Modulation Functions and Disorders.) Some, of course,

remain hypotheses. (See Chapter 23 , Is Sensory

Integration Effective? A Complicated Question

to End the Book.)

 Drawing from her doctoral study in educational psychology, Ayres developed tests to

measure the constructs associated with SI theory

CHAPTER 1 Sensory Integration: A. Jean Ayres’ Theory Revisited ■ 3

and examine their relationships. Many other

researchers have since replicated and extended

Ayres’ research—for the most part upholding

her fi ndings. (See also Chapter 15 , Advances in

Sensory Integration Research: Clinically Based

Research.)

 Based on the hypothesized relationships

among theoretical constructs, Ayres developed SI

therapy. When she observed changes in learning

and behavior in children who received the intervention, she hypothesized that those improvements refl ected improved SI and enhanced

neural functioning. Ayres ( 1972a, 1976 ) then

tested the effectiveness of SI therapy through

research, supporting her hypotheses. Several

other researchers subsequently tested the effectiveness of SI therapy, some supporting the

intervention and others questioning its effectiveness. We summarize some of that research

in Chapter 15 (Advances in Sensory Integration

Research: Clinically Based Research) and refl ect

on the mixed fi ndings in Chapter 23 (Is Sensory

Integration Effective? A Complicated Question

to End the Book).

 Ayres died more than 30 years ago. Although

numerous authors have proposed alternative terminology and represented SI theory in different

ways, the theory remains remarkably similar to

that which Ayres proposed. In this chapter, we

demonstrate the continuity of the theory.

 SI therapy, too, bears marked similarity

to that which Ayres implemented in her own

clinic. In creating a fi delity measure, Parham

and colleagues ( 2011 ) clarifi ed the criteria for SI

therapy. Those criteria are now applied in, and to,

research and practice. (See Chapter 14 , Distilling Sensory Integration Theory for Use: Making

Sense of the Complexity.) Research, although

not unequivocally demonstrating the effectiveness of the therapy, has made great strides in that

direction. The return to studying SI therapy in its

entirety—art and science—has made important

contributions to demonstrating effectiveness.

 This chapter provides an introduction to the

theory now known as Ayres Sensory Integration ® (ASI). We list the constructs associated

with the theory and illustrate their hypothesized

relationships. We describe two major categories of SI dysfunction: dyspraxia and sensory

modulation dysfunction. Within each of those

categories, we defi ne different subtypes: for

dyspraxia, diffi culties with vestibular bilateral

integration and sequencing (VBIS) and somatodyspraxia; for sensory modulation dysfunction,

over-responsivity (i.e., sensory defensiveness,

gravitational insecurity, and aversive responses

to movement) and under-responsivity. We offer

a model expanding Ayres’ conceptualization of

SI to explicitly include psychosocial sequelae.

We compare schematic representations created

by numerous authors illustrating SI theory in different ways. We describe the boundaries of the

theory and intervention with regard to the populations to whom it can be applied and the nature

of the intervention.

 In this chapter, we do not attempt to explain

the neurological underpinnings of SI. Neither

do we critique the assessments associated with

the theory, describe the intervention in detail, or

discuss the research examining its effectiveness.

We cover all that in chapters comprising the

subsequent seven sections: (1) Theory; (2) Sensory Integrative Dysfunction and Hypothesized

Neu roscience Underpinnings; (3) Assessment;

(4) Intervention; (5) Research; (6) Complementing and Extending the Theory and Its Application; (7) Applications.

Sensory Integrative Dysfunction:

Illustrating the Reasoning

CASE STUDY ■ JOSHUA

 Joshua has poor gross motor coordination and

diffi culty learning new motor tasks. Some of

the other children in his class play baseball on a

local team, but Josh cannot catch, throw, or bat

nearly well enough to be included. Although

he rides his bicycle everywhere, learning to

ride took more effort for him than for others.

However, he prefers riding the bicycle to a

skateboard, which he found “just too hard.”

 On standardized testing, Josh had diffi culty

discriminating touch, imitating postures, and

reproducing movement sequences that involve

coordinated use of both sides of the body. On

standard clinical observations, he had poor

posture and ineffective equilibrium reactions.

 Josh has no evidence of peripheral nervous

system or CNS damage and he is of average

intelligence. Therefore, because empirical evidence consistently links diminished ability to

discriminate body sensations with poor praxis,

4 ■ PART I Theoretical Constructs

we speculated that Josh ’ s problems were caused

by sensory integrative dysfunction. We further

speculated that sensory integrative therapy

would improve Josh ’ s ability to integrate sensation and plan actions. Sensory integrative

therapy involves active engagement in activities

providing a “just right challenge” and inherent

opportunities to take in enhanced tactile, vestibular, and proprioceptive sensations.

 We hypothesized that if, after a trial of intervention, Josh ’ s coordination seemed better, we

would attribute the changes to improved SI

and motor planning. Even without the ability

to directly observe CNS processing, we would

also hypothesize that improvements in processing were present.

Introduction to Sensory

Integration Theory

 Ayres ( 1972b ) defi ned sensory integration as

“the neurological process that organizes sensation from one ’ s own body and from the environment and makes it possible to use the body

effectively within the environment” (p. 11). Ayres

focused on the vestibular, proprioceptive, and

tactile systems—perhaps because she believed

that researchers had ignored them in favor of the

visual and auditory systems. Although the vestibular, tactile, and proprioceptive systems take

center stage in Ayres’ theory, she did not discount the importance of the visual and auditory

systems. In fact, Ayres ( 1972b, p . 73) indicated

that “the human being is not only a highly visual

animal, he is . . . so conscious of being visual

that the very word ‘perception’ is usually construed to mean visual perception” (italics added).

Nonetheless, Ayres felt that visual perception

was an end product and that the vestibular system

was its main foundation ( Sieg, 1988 ). Similarly,

Ayres studied the effect of sensory integrative

therapy on children with auditory-language diffi culties, believing that the relationship between

the vestibular and auditory systems meant they

might be good candidates for the intervention.

 Although Ayres ( 1979 ) included the auditory

and visual systems in her schematic representations of SI theory (see, for example, Fig. 1-3 ),

she never fully fl eshed out their contributions to

the theory. Currently, therapists implementing

sensory integrative therapy often also prescribe

PRACTICE WISDOM

Theory Is Not Fact

SI is a theory of brain–behavior relationships.

Theories are provisional statements that help to

• Explain why people behave in particular

ways

• Develop intervention to ameliorate related

diffi culties

• Predict how behavior will change because of

intervention

programs such as Interactive Metronome® and

Therapeutic Listening® that target the auditory

system (see Chapter 18 , Complementary Programs for Intervention). Similar to Ayres, we

include the visual and auditory systems in the

model we use throughout this text ( Fig. 1-6 )

and in Chapter 4 (Structure and Function of the

Sensory Systems). However, even now, SI theory

has not expanded to embrace these systems fully.

Postulates of Sensory

Integration Theory

 SI theory is not only about dysfunction and

intervention. It also postulates that effi cient SI

underlies learning. Thus, SI theory comprises

three broad postulates. The fi rst describes SI and

learning. The second defi nes sensory integrative

dysfunction. The third guides intervention.

 1. Learning, in the broadest sense, is dependent

on the ability to process and integrate

sensation and use it to plan and organize

behavior. This postulate has given rise to

the constructs included in the theory and

described throughout this book.

 2. A decreased ability to process and integrate

sensation may result in diffi culty producing

appropriate actions, which, in turn, may

interfere with learning and behavior. This

postulate has given rise to standardized

tests refl ecting the various constructs of

the theory. See Chapter 8 (Assessment of

Sensory Integration Functions Using the

Sensory Integration and Praxis Tests) and

 Chapter 9 (Using Clinical Observations

within the Evaluation Process) and also

 Dunn ( 2014 ), and Parham, Ecker, Kuhaneck,

Henry, and Glennon ( 2010 ).

CHAPTER 1 Sensory Integration: A. Jean Ayres’ Theory Revisited ■ 5

 3. Sensations generated and integrated in the

context of a “just right challenge” contribute

to improved CNS processing, thereby

enhancing learning and behavior. This

postulate has given rise to sensory integrative

therapy. See also Chapter 12 (The Art of

Therapy) and Chapter 13 (The Science of

Intervention: Creating Direct Intervention

from Theory).

Illustrating Sensory

Integration Theory

 Schematic representations are a good way of

portraying the essence of a theory by displaying

the relationships among the constructs. Creators

of schematics select the constructs and illustrate

their relationships in the ways that make sense

to them and emphasize the points they fi nd most

salient. Thus, different authors often represent

the same theory in slightly different ways.

Sensory Integration Theory

and Learning

 In Figure 1-1 , we depict the contribution of SI

theory to learning, very simplistically, as a circular process. The process begins with taking in

sensation and culminates with sensory feedback.

Feedback, together with new sensory intakes,

in turn, initiate a new cycle of learning. In

 Figure 1-2 , we broaden the schematic shown in

 Figure 1-1 , overlaying the dysfunction, evaluation, and intervention postulates of SI theory on

the intake, processing, and feedback circle.

 Ayres ( 1979 ) focused on the contribution of

SI to learning in the complex schematic representation shown in Figure 1-3 . At the far left of

the schematic, she listed all the senses (i.e., the

FIGURE 1-1 A schematic representation of the learning component of SI theory.

FIGURE 1-2 A simple representation of the three

postulates of SI theory.

Intervention

Feedback

Evaluation

Sensory

integration

Sensory

intake

Adaptive

interaction

Hypothesized

Site of Problem

Planning

and

organizing

behavior

6 ■ PART I Theoretical Constructs

sources of sensory intake and feedback). Moving

to the right, Ayres described increasingly complex

outcomes related to effi cient processing of sensation, labeling them “integration of inputs” and

“end products.” On the schematic, Ayres used

brackets to associate the outcomes with relevant

senses. Her schematic clearly shows the hypothesized contributions of the vestibular, proprioceptive, and tactile systems to learning but also

includes the auditory and visual systems. The

further to the right that one moves on Figure 1-3 ,

the more distant the inputs from the simple sensations and the more that other factors potentially

come into play. For example, ability to concentrate, located at the far right, depends on all the

senses, as shown by the bracket. But ability to

concentrate also depends on many other factors

(e.g., cognition, interest, and environment).

Sensory Integrative Dysfunction

 In Figure 1-4 , we present a simple schematic

representation of the two major categories of

sensory integrative dysfunction: sensory modulation dysfunction and dyspraxia. As the diagram

shows, individuals with sensory integrative dysfunction can have one or both types of dysfunction. This simple representation is a broad-brush

illustration of sensory integrative dysfunction,

showing the big categories but lacking detail

about subcategories.

 Miller and her colleagues ( Miller, Anzalone,

Lane, Cermak, & Osten, 2007 ) also created a

schematic representation of sensory integrative

dysfunction, which they referred to as a “nosology” (i.e., a system for classifying diseases).

However, rather than use the label sensory integrative dysfunction, they coined the term sensory

FIGURE 1-3 Ayres’ ( 1979 ) schematic representation of SI theory. Sample items of the Sensory Integration

and the Child, 25th Anniversary Edition copyright © 2005, by Western Psychological Services. Reprinted by

permission of the publisher, Western Psychological Services. Not to be reprinted in whole or in part for any

additional purpose without the expressed, written permission of the publisher (rights@wpspublish.com). All

rights reserved.

CHAPTER 1 Sensory Integration: A. Jean Ayres’ Theory Revisited ■ 7

processing disorder. (See Fig. 1-5 .) The constructs included in the nosology are remarkably

similar to those that Ayres described in SI theory

and, in fact, were determined largely based on

 Ayres ( 1972c, 1989 ) and other researchers (e.g.,

 DeGangi, 2000 ; Dunn, 2001 ; Mulligan, 1996,

1998, 2000 ) who sought to build on Ayres’ fi ndings ( Miller et al., 2007 ). The nosology confi rms

that it is possible to change a name without

changing the basic theory.

 Miller et al. ’ s ( 2007 ) nosology is simple and,

in many ways, elegant. However, one diffi culty

with the nosology is that it fails to depict explicit

links between sensory systems and behaviors.

For example, the links between posture and

the vestibular and proprioceptive systems, or

between praxis and aspects of sensory discrimination, seen in Ayres’ model are not included in

Miller et al. ’ s nosology.

FIGURE 1-5 Miller et al. ’ s ( 2007 ) nosology of sensory processing disorders. Republished with permission of

AOTA, from Concept Evolution in Sensory Integration: A Proposed Nosology for Diagnosis by Lucy Jane Miller,

Marie E. Anzalone, Shelly J. Lane, Sharon A. Cermak, and Elizabeth T. Osten; volume 61, March/April 2007.

 We created the schematic shown in Figure 1-6

to represent hypothesized relationships between

the sensory systems and behavioral manifestations of sensory integrative dysfunction.

Although this schematic focuses on dysfunction rather than function, Figure 1-6 is organized similarly to Ayres’ schematic ( Fig. 1-3 ).

However, Figure 1-6 is read outward from the

center column where CNS processing of sensation is depicted. Indicators of poor SI and praxis

are shown on the right, and indicators of sensory

modulation dysfunction appear on the left. This

enables us to show the two manifestations of

sensory integrative dysfunction: dyspraxia and

sensory modulation dysfunction, and their relationship to the processing of sensation. As shown

in Figure 1-4 , an individual can have one or both

of these broad categories of sensory integrative

dysfunction.

 In Figure 1-6 , the closer the columns are to the

center, the more direct the relationship with processing of sensation. For example, postural ocular

control appears in the column just to the right of

the center column (i.e., on the praxis side). Processing of vestibular and proprioceptive sensation is related directly to postural ocular control.

In fact, in SI theory, a meaningful cluster of postural ocular reactions is regarded as a direct manifestation of vestibular proprioceptive processing.

(See also Chapter 4 , Structure and Function of

the Sensory Systems, and Chapter 9 , Using Clinical Observations within the Evaluation Process.)

In other words, although we cannot see poor

vestibular proprioceptive processing, we see a FIGURE 1-4 Simplifi ed representation of

manifestation of SI dysfunction.

Dyspraxia Poor sensory

modulation

SI

dysfunction

8 ■ PART I Theoretical Constructs

meaningful cluster of behaviors that defi ne poor

postural control and we interpret these behaviors

as refl ecting diffi culties with processing vestibular proprioceptive sensations. Similarly, body

scheme (i.e., the sense of where body parts are

in relation to one another) is tied directly to the

processing of sensation although it is not readily

observable. We interpret actions in which children seem unaware of the relationship of body

parts to one another or to objects as refl ecting

poor body scheme.

 As we move further to the right side

on Figure 1-6 , similar to Ayres’ schematic

( Fig. 1-3 ), the columns contain constructs tied

less directly to processing of sensation. Those

constructs are complex and many factors contribute to their quality. Defi cits in praxis (i.e.,

VBIS, somatodyspraxia) comprise one column.

The columns further to the right contain abstract

constructs common in individuals with poor

praxis but which, by themselves, are not indicators of sensory integrative dysfunction: poor

self-effi cacy, avoidance, and other similar items.

That is, many factors besides sensory integrative dysfunction can cause poor self-effi cacy,

and so on.

 In Figure 1-6 , indicators of poor sensory

modulation appear in the columns on the left.

The closer the columns to the center, the more

directly they refl ect processing of sensation. Each

sensory system may be associated directly with

over- or under-responsivity. The constructs that

appear further to the left (e.g., sensory-related

challenges with attention, withdrawal, seeking,

and, ultimately, challenges with occupational

engagement) are common in individuals with

sensory modulation defi cits but they are complex

and infl uenced by multiple factors.

 Challenges with occupational engagement

comprise the columns at the farthest left and the

farthest right. Although some of the labels within

those columns are the same, we expect the nature

of the challenges that result from poor modulation to differ from those related to dyspraxia. For

example, both groups of children might have diffi culty with dressing or completing school work.

The challenges for a child with poor modulation

might stem from diffi culties maintaining attention, whereas those for a child with dyspraxia

would refl ect the motor challenge. Similarly, both

groups might have diffi culty with self-esteem or

self-effi cacy. Children with a sensory modulation

FIGURE 1-6 Complex schematic representation of sensory integrative dysfunction.

Autonomic Limbic Reticular Thalamus Cerebellum Basal Ganglia Cortex

Behavioral

consequences

Indicators of

poor sensory

modulation

 Over-

 responsivity

• Aversive

 and

 defensive

 reactions

 Under-

 responsivity

 • Poor

 registration

Inadequate

CNS integration

and processing

of sensation

Visual

Vestibular

Tactile

[lnteroception]

Auditory

Olfactory

Gustatory

Proprioception

Indicators of poor sensory

integration and praxis

Poor

postural-ocular

control

 Poor sensory

 discrimination

• Tactile

• Proprioception

• Vestibular

• Visual

• Auditory

Poor body

scheme

Sensory reactivity

Sensory perception

VBIS

Behavioral

consequences

Poor selfefficacy,

self-esteem

Sensory

seeking

Poor

organization

Poor gross,

fine, and

visual motor

coordination

Avoidance of

engagement

in motor

activities

Clowning

Occupational Engagement Challenges

Occupational Engagement Challenges

Sensoryrelated

challenges

with attention,

regulation,

affect, activity

Somatodyspraxia

Poor selfefficacy,

self-esteem

Withdrawal

from, and

avoidance of,

sensory

experiences

Sensory

seeking

CHAPTER 1 Sensory Integration: A. Jean Ayres’ Theory Revisited ■ 9

disorder, for example, may have decreased

self-esteem because they are often in trouble for

misbehaving, and may come to see themselves

as bad or worthless (i.e., low self-esteem) or

not as capable as others (i.e., low self-effi cacy).

Children with dyspraxia, on the other hand, may

experience low self-effi cacy because they perceive their motor skills as inferior to those of

peers and low self-esteem because they believe

that people with poor motor skills are less valuable than people with good motor skills. Sensory

seeking also appears on the extremes of both

sides of the model as a behavior associated with

sensory under-responsivity or poor body scheme,

when a child seeks unusually great amounts of

sensation to augment the sense of where his or

her body parts are in space.

HERE ’ S THE POINT

• The initial model of SI was developed by A.

Jean Ayres, and it depicts SI function.

• The current model, used throughout this

text, is one depicting two broad categories of

dysfunction, dyspraxia and sensory modulation

disorders.

The Constructs

 The two primary categories of sensory integrative

dysfunction, dyspraxia and sensory modulation

dysfunction, deserve some further explanation.

In Table 1-1 , we briefl y defi ne and describe the

constructs associated with each category.

TABLE 1-1 Constructs Associated with Sensory Integrative Dysfunction

CONSTRUCTS ASSOCIATED WITH DYSPRAXIA

Construct Brief Description Hypothesized Cause Related Chapters

VBIS Diffi culty planning and using

the two sides of the body

in a coordinated fashion

and sequencing anticipatory

(feedforward-dependent) motor

actions (e.g., Schmidt & Lee, 2011 )

Poor central processing

of vestibular and

proprioceptive sensations,

seen as poor posturalocular control

5: Praxis and Dyspraxia

9: Using Clinical

Observations within the

Evaluation Process; for

assessment of bilateral

coordination and projected

action sequences

Somatodyspraxia More severe form of dyspraxia

than VBIS

Defi cits in body scheme

resulting from poor

sensory discrimination,

esp. tactile, proprioceptive,

or vestibular

5: Praxis and Dyspraxia;

for a thorough description

of somatodyspraxia

Poor posturalocular control

Basis for VBIS and sometimes for

somatodyspraxia. Seen in

 • Low extensor muscle tone

 • Decreased ability to assume

anti-gravity postures

 • Poor proximal stability

 • Poor equilibrium

 • Depressed postrotary nystagmus

Outward manifestation

of defi cits in vestibular or

proprioceptive processing

7: Sensory Discrimination

Functions and Disorders

9: Using Clinical

Observations within the

Evaluation Process

Poor body

scheme

Defi cits in the internal map

representing the spatial

relationship of body parts

Poor discrimination of

tactile and proprioceptive

(i.e., somatosensory)

sensations

4: Structure and Function

of the Sensory Systems

5: Praxis and Dyspraxia

Poor sensory

discrimination

Poor interpretation of the

spatiotemporal characteristics

of sensation (i.e., Where? How

intense? Direction of movement?)

Poor CNS processing of

inputs in any sensory

system

4: Structure and Function

of the Sensory Systems

7: Sensory Discrimination

Functions and Disorders

10 ■ PART I Theoretical Constructs

 Dyspraxia

 In sensory integrative theory, dyspraxia refers to

diffi culty planning new movements stemming

from poor body scheme that, in turn, results from

defi cits in processing vestibular, proprioceptive,

or tactile sensation. Ayres ( 1985 ) described ideation (i.e., the formation of ideas for action) as

a cognitive component of praxis. Two decades

later, May-Benson ( 2001, 2005 ; May-Benson &

Cermak, 2007 ) explored ideation and sensoryintegrative-based dyspraxia more fully. And

although it seems clear that some children with

dyspraxia, particularly those with more severe

forms, have diffi culty with ideation, to date no

researchers have included data refl ecting ideation in a factor analytic study examining its

relationship to other constructs of SI theory or

situating it within types of dyspraxia.

 In this text, we describe two types of dyspraxia: VBIS dysfunction and somatodyspraxia.

As you see in Table 1-1 , we hypothesize that

VBIS is a less severe practic defi cit than somatodyspraxia. We base that on research suggesting that praxis is a single construct (e.g., Lai,

Fisher, Magalhaes, & Bundy, 1996 ; Mailloux

et al., 2011 ; Mulligan, 1996, 1998, 2000 ) and on

extensive practical experience. The argument for

praxis as a single construct with VBIS as less

severe and somatodyspraxia as more severe is

logical given that children with somatodyspraxia

have diffi culty with both simple (feedbackdependent) and complex (feedforward-dependent)

PRACTICE WISDOM

When actions are controlled by feedback, they

can be adjusted in response to that feedback.

For example, when a child stops a ball with his

or her foot before kicking it, the child can adjust

the direction of the kick in response to the tactile

and proprioceptive feedback received through his

or her foot and leg. When movements are under

feedforward-control, a child must move his or her

hand or foot to a particular location before the

object on which he or she will act arrives at that

location. Feedforward-dependent actions cannot

be adjusted after the command to execute the

movement has been issued. So, the child who runs

to meet the ball and kicks it in mid-stride cannot

adjust the kick once it has been initiated. Not surprisingly, many feedforward-dependent actions are

bilateral.

Sugden and his colleagues ( Henderson &

Sugden, 1992 ; Keogh & Sugden, 1985 ) offered a

simple model for gauging the degree to which an

action is under relative feedback or feedforwardcontrol. These investigators indicated that control

(feedforward vs. feedback) is a function of movement of both the child and any object(s) on which

the child is acting (e.g., a ball). The more or the faster

that the child or an object moves, the more control

must be feedforward-dependent. Feedforwarddependent actions are more diffi cult than feedbackdependent actions. Here is an adapted version of

Sugden ’ s model.

In the fi gure, a line connecting points representing relative movement of a child and an object

passes through a point on the line illustrating the

degree to which the action is feedforward- or

feedback-dependent. Clearly, this diagram is overly

simplistic but it can be useful for understanding the

motor planning diffi culties of children with dyspraxia and for grading the diffi culty of intervention

activities.

Control of movement as a function of the mover and the object.

Feedback

B A C

Object movement

(degree, speed)

Person movement

(degree, speed)

Feedforward

Control of

movement

CHAPTER 1 Sensory Integration: A. Jean Ayres’ Theory Revisited ■ 11

actions whereas children with VBIS primarily

have diffi culty with the high-level feedforwarddependent tasks.

Sensory Modulation Dysfunction

 The concepts of sensory modulation and

sensory modulation dysfunction are somewhat

abstract. Although the term modulation is familiar to many therapists, its precise meaning can

be somewhat elusive. Ayres ( 1979 ), who fi rst

applied the concept to SI theory, defi ned modulation as the CNS ’ s regulation of its own activity.

Engineers liken modulation to tuning a radio to

the amplitude and frequency of the sound waves

emitted by a station. When the amplitude and

frequency detected by the radio tuner match

those of the station ’ s sound waves, the station

comes in clearly. However, when the tuner is not

properly modulated, the radio is rendered ineffective. Individuals who have diffi culty modulating sensation behave as though the amplitude of

their response is consistently greater or less than

that of most individuals, decreasing the effectiveness of their performance. This is illustrated in

 Figure 1-7 .

 In this text, we describe two types of sensory

modulation dysfunction (i.e., over-responsivity

and under-responsivity). The responses of a child

who is overly responsive to sensation are of

greater amplitude than expected. We label overresponsivity to tactile or auditory sensations as

defensiveness (i.e., a fi ght-or-fl ight reaction) and

to vestibular sensation as gravitational insecurity

FIGURE 1-7 Schematic representation of modulation and modulation defi cits.

HERE ’ S THE EVIDENCE

For more than four decades, factor analyses of data

from the Sensory Integration and Praxis Tests ( Ayres,

 1989 ) and its precursor, the Southern California

Sensory Integration Tests ( Ayres, 1972c ), along with

the Southern California Postrotary Nystagmus Test

( Ayres, 1975 ), yielded patterns of test scores that

describe sensory-integrative-based dyspraxia ( Ayres,

1965, 1966a, 1966b, 1969, 1972d, 1977, 1989 ;

Ayres, Mailloux, & Wendler, 1987 ; Mailloux et al.,

 2011 ; Mulligan, 1996, 1998, 2000 ). Ayres found

that tests requiring bilateral integration, sequencing of fi nger and arm movements, and postural

control typically loaded together on a factor known

most recently as VBIS. 1

 She also found that tests

of imitation, oral praxis, tactile discrimination, and

proprioception consistently loaded together; she

called these somatopraxis. More recent researchers

( Mailloux et al., 2011 ; Mulligan, 1996, 1998, 2000 )

found slightly different loadings of tests, although

they found relationships among all factors. Mailloux

et al. ( 2011 ) labeled their practic factors “Visuodyspraxia and Somatodyspraxia” and “Vestibular and

Proprioceptive Bilateral Integration and Sequencing” (VPBIS). VPBIS contained loadings of both

sensory and motor tests, but Visuodyspraxia and

Somatodyspraxia did not. Mulligan ( 1998 ) similarly

failed to fi nd loadings of sensory and motor on the

same factor. She concluded, cogently, that it is best

always to describe the nature of a particular child ’ s

sensory integrative dysfunction. For example, a

child with both dyspraxia and poor somatosensory

discrimination is best described using both labels.

1

VBIS has been known by several names: for example, just BIS, and Postural and Bilateral Integration (PBI).

12 ■ PART I Theoretical Constructs

(i.e., fear) or aversive (i.e., autonomic) responses.

Under-responsivity refers to reactions of lessthan-expected amplitude. We can see them in

any sensory system. It is worth noting that underresponsivity can be diffi cult to differentiate

from poor discrimination. Clinically, therapists

have also identifi ed fl uctuating responsivity, a

term used with children whose responses are

sometimes greater-than-expected and sometimes

less-than-expected in any sensory system or

across systems.

 Review Table 1-1 for a description of the

major constructs associated with SI theory. We

begin with indicators of poor SI and praxis

and then move to indicators of poor sensory

modulation.

HERE ’ S THE POINT

• Problems in sensory discrimination may lead

to poor postural-ocular control and poor body

scheme which, in turn, contribute to dyspraxia

(i.e., VBIS and somatodyspraxia).

• Sensory modulation dysfunction is seen

as persistent over- or under-responding to

sensations.

Uniting Sensory Integration

with Psychosocial Constructs

 Kielhofner and Fisher ( 1991 ) felt that viewing

poor self-esteem as simply a byproduct of

sensory integrative dysfunction failed to consider

suffi ciently how a child ’ s view of self emerges

and how self-perception infl uences that child ’ s

behavior. They indicated that psychosocial events

are at least as complex as SI and pointed to the

need for a sophisticated view. Kielhofner and

Fisher illustrated the complexity of the relationship between sensory integrative dysfunction and

common psychosocial sequelae with the story of

Joe, a 9-year-old boy up to bat in a Little League

practice session.

CASE STUDY ■ JOE

 Joe ’ s brain appeared to lack the ability to integrate sensation from his body and the environment effectively and effi ciently. His diffi culty

integrating sensation seemed to explain his

struggles with planning and producing motor

actions that caused him to appear clumsy. But

what does SI theory tell us about how or what

Joe felt? Joe deeply wanted to play baseball

well. But he felt extremely frightened as the

pitcher got ready to throw the ball. Joe knew

the challenge was to meet the pitched ball with

the swing of his bat, but he did not know how to

do it. He had little awareness of how it should

HERE ’ S THE EVIDENCE

Statistical evidence of sensory modulation dysfunction was not available until the 1990s when

formal tests were fi rst developed. Before that

time, a diagnosis of sensory modulation dysfunction was based on observation and informal

interview. For example, Ayres’ ( 1972c, 1989 ) statistical analyses included only her observation of

a child ’ s discomfort in response to being touched

during testing, which she labeled tactile defensiveness. Now, standardized tests exist that,

although not purely measures of modulation

or even of dysfunction, contain items refl ecting processing vulnerabilities (e.g., under- and

over-responsiveness, sensory seeking) in multiple sensory systems. Identifi cation of fl uctuating responsivity across sensory systems requires

examining and comparing scores for sensory

domains. We do not have a standardized test

that clearly identifi es fl uctuating responsivity

within a single sensory system.

Two “families” of tests are used most commonly: (1) Sensory Profi le 2 ( Dunn, 2014 ) with

versions for infants, toddlers, and children, and

the Adolescent/Adult Sensory Profi le ( Brown

& Dunn, 2002 ) and (2) Sensory Processing

Measures ( Parham, Ecker, Kuhaneck, Henry, &

Glennon, 2010 ). Factor analytical studies of data

from these various parent- and self-report measures have revealed patterns of sensory modulation or sensory modulation dysfunction (e.g.,

 Dunn, 1994, 2014 ; Dunn & Brown, 1997 ; Dunn

& Westman, 1997 ). More recently, researchers (e.g., A. E. Lane, Young, Baker, & Angley,

 2010 ; S. J. Lane, Reynolds, & Dumenci, 2012 ;

S. J. Lane, Reynolds, & Thacker, 2010 ; Reynolds,

Bendixen, Lawrence, & Lane, 2011 ; Reynolds,

Lane, & Thacker, 2011 ) have paired parent and

self-report measures with behavioral tests and

physiological measures in an attempt to gain

further understanding of sensory modulation

and sensory modulation dysfunction.

CHAPTER 1 Sensory Integration: A. Jean Ayres’ Theory Revisited ■ 13

feel to swing the bat and hit the ball. What he

could feel was the eyes of his peers bearing

down on him as the ball raced in his direction.

Joe became increasingly aware of an aching

feeling in the pit of his stomach; his anxiety

was acute and distressing. Joe had a deep and

pervasive feeling that he was “no good” and

that he would not be able to hit the ball.

 Joe ’ s emotional state manifested as overarousal and anxiety. As the ball approached,

it seemed to disappear from sight and he was

unaware of his relationship to it in time and

space. He swung the bat almost in self-defense

and in the vain hope that somehow it would

connect with the ball. But he missed widely

and the whole performance had a tragicomic

appearance. A chorus of jeers and laughter from

his peers painfully drove home his error. And

this was not a new experience; Joe ’ s discomfort

with using his body for any of the coordinated

actions required in sports was familiar to him.

The harder he tried, the more diffi cult it was

to get things right. For Joe, not being able to

execute motor actions as he wished and feeling

uncomfortable around peers as his performance

missed the mark was a familiar and uncomfortable experience.

 In Joe ’ s case, we may speculate that ineffi -

cient processing of sensation was responsible

for the quality of his motor performance. But

we can hardly argue that Joe ’ s diffi culty processing sensation, and nothing else, caused his

poor performance. Clearly, Joe ’ s psychological state had something to do with how he

performed. What went on as Joe missed the

ball was much more than just a case of poor

SI and uncoordinated motor behavior. Neither

Joe ’ s performance nor his experience could be

captured adequately by explanations grounded

only in SI. Rather, the psychosocial experience

was an equally important part of what Joe did

and felt.

 Kielhofner and Fisher ( 1991 ) cited DiJoseph

 ( 1982 ) in pointing out that when a therapist focuses only on SI, paying little attention to psychosocial phenomena—or vice versa—intervention

is necessarily incomplete. A therapeutic approach

that appreciated how they were interrelated had

obvious advantages over a narrow or fragmented

approach. Thus, in an attempt to address the very

real concerns that Kielhofner and Fisher discussed, Fisher and Murray ( 1991 ) adopted a spiraling model often used in occupational therapy

theory. Their Spiral Process of Self-Actualization

(see Fig. 1-8 ) blended SI theory with constructs

drawn from the Model of Human Occupation to

explore self-actualization.

The Spiral Process of Self-Actualization

 In Fisher and Murray ’ s ( 1991 ) Spiral Process of

Self-Actualization, SI theory is depicted in the

medium gray spiral, and concepts drawn from

the Model of Human Occupation are depicted

in the white spiral. Inner drive, which provides

the impetus to become involved in meaningful

activities, is at the base of the spiral. Fisher and

Murray defi ned “meaningful” as having signifi -

cance, value, or purpose. For an activity to be

meaningful, a child must be in control and able

to make sense of the experience.

 Actively taking in sensation (sensory intake)

is an early step in the sensory integrative

process. There are many sources of sensation,

including the physical and social environments

(represented by arrows labeled environment )

and production and outcome feedback. Production feedback arises from the body and informs

the child how it felt to move; outcome feedback

arises from actions that produced a change in the

environment.

 Sensations are integrated (i.e., combined),

enabling a child to act effectively and effi ciently

on objects in the environment. Ayres referred to

these successful actions as adaptive interactions.

Adaptive interactions give rise to production and

outcome feedback. Adaptive (and sometimes

not-so-adaptive) interactions are the behaviors

we observe during evaluation.

 Planning an adaptive interaction means

knowing “what to do” and organizing “how to do

it.” Successful plans depend on a desire to participate, a suffi cient body scheme developed from

previous production feedback, and knowledge of

outcome feedback from previous adaptive interactions. Production and outcome feedback are

important to planning and ultimately to learning.

After a neuronal model of an action is developed, it can be used as the basis for new, more

complex interactions. Thus, Fisher and Murray

added a third loop (dark lines) to their spirals to

depict neuronal models (see Fig. 1-8 ).

14 ■ PART I Theoretical Constructs

FIGURE 1-8 Spiral process of SI.

 The white loop of the spiral refl ects a core

assumption of occupational therapy: Humans

have an occupational nature. According to Fisher

and Murray ( 1991 ), the addition of this loop to the

traditional depiction of SI theory shows its place

in the greater context of occupational science.

Adaptive interactions are basic to occupational

behavior. Two core assumptions of occupational

science are that humans have an innate need to

participate in occupation and that occupation is

intrinsically motivating. In turn, humans develop

meaning, satisfaction, confi dence, self-control,

CHAPTER 1 Sensory Integration: A. Jean Ayres’ Theory Revisited ■ 15

and a sense of mastery from participation ( White,

 1959 ). Thus, the impetus for planning and organizing adaptive interactions includes both sensation and volitional factors (e.g., motivation,

self-direction).

 Adaptive interactions imply that individuals

feel a certain amount of control over the task and

the environment. Children who develop a sense

of mastery also develop belief in their abilities

(i.e., belief in skill, self-effi cacy ). Self-effi cacy

enables children to self-direct. They become

motivated to explore their capacity through planning and producing adaptive interactions and

participating in meaningful occupations.

 In summary, through a spiral process of

self-actualization, SI, beliefs about the self, and

volition, together, contribute to adaptive interactions. In turn, adaptive interactions yield organized and effective occupational behaviors (e.g.,

self-care, play, academic performance). As children develop control over the environment and

belief in their own skills, interactions with the

environment become more meaningful and satisfying and children are likely to want to engage

in similar actions and occupations repeatedly.

Importantly, however, increased belief in skill

does not necessarily or automatically accompany

the ability to produce higher quality adaptive

interactions. Therapists must ensure that children

know that they have developed a new skill.

HERE ’ S THE POINT

• Blending psychosocial constructs with those

inherent in SI theory, as is represented in

the spiral model presented, provides a more

complete picture of the child ’ s strengths and

needs, and lays a broader foundation for

intervention.

• In the spiral process of self-actualization, SI,

beliefs about the self, and volition are seen

as contributing to adaptive environmental

interactions. Adaptive interactions are the

foundation for effective occupational

behaviors.

All Theories Are Based

on Underlying Assumptions

 Accepting any theory means accepting the

assumptions that underlie it. The assumptions

underlying SI theory relate to its neural and

behavioral bases. In Table 1-2 , we describe four

assumptions associated with sensory integrative

therapy. We explain those assumptions and their

rationale. We also list chapters in this text where

readers can fi nd more information.

Boundaries of Sensory

Integration Theory

and Intervention

 Ayres envisioned the application of SI theory and

intervention with particular populations: children with learning disabilities or autism. More

recently, Parham and colleagues ( Parham et al.,

 2011 ) defi ned specifi c criteria for sensory integrative therapy. Nonetheless, both practitioners

and researchers sometimes apply SI theory in

ways that are outside its boundaries. When therapists apply SI theory in other-than-intended

ways, they must always proceed with particular

caution. When researchers say they are investigating the effi cacy of SI, they must ensure that

their procedures are faithful to the principles of

the theory. When they critique the theory, particularly through meta-analysis or systematic

review, they must be certain that the papers they

include actually refl ect SI and not, as often seems

to happen, prescribed techniques that include

enhanced sensation but do not meet Parham

et al. ’ s criteria. See also Chapter 14 (Distilling

Sensory Integration Theory for Use: Making

Sense of the Complexity) and Chapter 23

(Is Sensory Integration Effective? A Complicated

Question to End the Book).

Boundaries and the Population

 SI theory is intended to explain problems associated with dyspraxia or sensory modulation

disorders. A diagnosis of sensory integrative

dysfunction requires evidence of defi cits in the

central processing of vestibular, proprioceptive,

or tactile sensation that are not attributable to

frank peripheral nervous system or CNS damage

or associated with cognitive defi cits. Although

 Ayres ( 1972b ) wrote initially about the diffi -

culties of children with learning disorders, she

( Ayres, 1979 ; Ayres & Tickle, 1980 ) later focused

on children with autism. In the ensuing decades,

we have learned unequivocally that many (or

16 ■ PART I Theoretical Constructs

TABLE 1-2 Four Assumptions Associated with Sensory Integrative Therapy

ASSUMPTION EXPLANATION RELATED CHAPTERS

The CNS has

neuroplasticity.

Plasticity refers to the ability of brain structures to change.

Ayres hypothesized that sensory integrative therapy could

effect changes in the brain because of plasticity. Although

Ayres emphasized the structural and behavioral plasticity

of the young brain, there is ample evidence that plasticity

is also present in the adult brain. Much of the support for

plasticity and SI therapy is drawn from studies of the impact

of enriched environments on the structure and function of

the nervous system in animals (see, for example, Reynolds,

Lane, & Richards, 2010 ). 

18: Complementary Programs

for Intervention; for review

of literature on sensorimotorbased plasticity

21: Planning and

Implementing Intervention: A

Case Example of a Child with

Autism

The brain

functions as an

integrated whole.

Ayres felt that SI occurred mainly in subcortical centers

whereas cortical centers were responsible for abstraction,

perception, reasoning, language, and learning. We

now know that both cortical and subcortical structures

contribute to SI.

4: Structure and Function of

the Sensory Systems

5: Praxis and Dyspraxia

Adaptive

interactions are

critical to the

development of SI.

The CNS is an open system, capable of self-regulating, selforganizing, and changing. Adaptive interactions promote

SI, and the quality of a child ’ s actions refl ects SI. Active

movements produce sensory feedback that forms the basis

for neuronal models of “how it felt” to move. Knowledge

of the outcome of an action forms the basis for memories

of “what was achieved.” Neuronal models, derived from

production and outcome feedback, form the basis for

planning increasingly complex adaptive interactions ( Schmidt

& Lee, 2011 ).

5: Praxis and Dyspraxia

Children have

an inner drive to

develop SI through

participation in

sensorimotor

activities.

Ayres believed that all children have an inner drive to

master their bodies and the environment. According to

Ayres, inner drive is seen in excitement, confi dence, and

effort. She felt children with sensory integrative dysfunction

sometimes seem to have little evidence of inner drive.

Intervention leads to stronger evidence of the inner drive

to seek out self-actualizing activities that, in turn, enhance

SI ( Ayres , 1972b ). Fisher and Murray ’ s Spiral Model of SelfActualization, described previously, adds to this fundamental

assumption.

12: The Art of Therapy;

for in-depth depiction of

capturing inner drive

most) children with autism process sensation

abnormally ( American Psychiatric Association,

 2013 ). Several researchers (e.g., Baranek, Foster,

& Berkson, 1997 ) as well as people diagnosed

with autism (e.g., Grandin & Scariano, 1986 )

related poor processing of sensation to autism.

Recent research utilizing rigorous designs including sophisticated case reports ( Schaaf, Hunt, &

Benevides, 2012 ), multiple baseline single case

methodologies ( Bulkeley, Bundy, Roberts, &

Einfeld, 2016 ), and quasi-experimental or experimental designs ( Pfeiffer, Koenig, Kinnealey,

Sheppard, & Henderson, 2011 ; Schaaf et al.,

 2013 ) have demonstrated decreases in problematic or stereotypic behaviors and increases in

attainment of specifi c goals.

 Sensory integrative dysfunction is a diagnosis of exclusion. That is, it is used to explain

dysfunction in motor planning or sensory modulation that cannot be explained by frank CNS

damage, genetic issues, or other diagnostic conditions. Children with a range of developmental

disorders may have defi cits in functions typically

associated with sensory integrative dysfunction.

However, SI theory is not intended to explain,

for example, the depressed postrotary nystagmus, low muscle tone, poor proximal stability,

or poor equilibrium experienced by children with

Down syndrome or hearing loss. Their problems

are more clearly attributed to abnormalities of

the cerebellum ( Nommensen & Maas, 1993 ) or

damage to the eighth cranial nerve, respectively.

CHAPTER 1 Sensory Integration: A. Jean Ayres’ Theory Revisited ■ 17

Boundaries and Intervention

 The boundaries of SI theory also apply to intervention. Sensory integrative therapy involves

taking in sensation actively in the context of

meaningful, self-directed, adaptive interactions.

The emphasis is on the integration of vestibular, proprioceptive, and tactile sensations and

the promotion of posture, bilateral integration,

praxis, or sensory modulation. Many direct intervention programs referred to as SI probably are

more appropriately referred to as sensorimotor

because, although they promote engagement

in activities that involve sensory experiences,

they are adult- rather than child-directed. Some

involve only sensory stimulation because they

involve passive application of sensation in a prescribed manner. Parham ’ s and colleagues’ ( 2011 )

criteria defi ne sensory integrative therapy (i.e.,

ASI). (See also Chapter 12 , The Art of Therapy,

and Chapter 13 , The Science of Intervention:

Creating Direct Intervention from Theory.)

Boundaries and Critique

 Not infrequently, critics apply the term sensory

integration inappropriately to interventions that

are purely sensory stimulation. Consider, for

example, Lang et al. ( 2012 ), who published a

systematic review entitled “Sensory Integration

Therapy for Autism Spectrum Disorders.” In

reality, 9 of the 25 studies they reviewed evaluated the effectiveness of weighted vests, an

example of passive application of tactile sensation

(i.e., sensory stimulation), not of SI. Nonetheless, Lang et al. concluded, quite inappropriately given the papers included in their review,

that there is insuffi cient evidence to support

sensory integrative therapy for children with

autism spectrum disorder (ASD). When reading

or responding to such systematic reviews, we

must consider carefully the nature of the individual papers included. Do they actually meet the

criteria for SI? Neither publication in a credible

peer-reviewed journal nor mention of Parham et

al. ’ s fi delity measure in the introduction to their

paper meant that the studies Lang et al. reviewed

met the criteria for SI. When researchers such as

Lang et al. conclude, based on studies that are

not SI, that sensory integrative therapy is not

effective, they simply fuel controversy inappropriately. (See also Chapter 15 , Advances in

Sensory Integration Research: Clinically Based

Research, and Chapter 17 , Using Sensory Integration Theory in Coaching.)

HERE ’ S THE POINT

• Understanding the boundaries of SI theory and

intervention is essential.

• Constructs consistent with SI theory are

useful in explaining the problems associated

with dyspraxia and sensory modulation

disorders. They are not intended to explain all

developmental disorders.

• SI intervention elements have been captured

in the fi delity measure developed by Parham

and colleagues. They importantly include

meaningful, self-directed, and adaptive

interactions that are child directed and

focused. Other sensory-based approaches may

not include these central “ingredients.”

• When considering intervention effectiveness

studies, we must all remain vigilant that the

intervention under scrutiny adheres to the

essential features of ASI ® .

Summary and Conclusions

 In this chapter, we presented an overview of SI

theory and sensory integrative dysfunction, comparing and contrasting two main categories of

dysfunction: dyspraxia and sensory modulation

dysfunction. We distinguished between two types

of dyspraxia (i.e., defi cits in bilateral integration

and sequencing, somatodyspraxia) and two types

of sensory modulation dysfunction (i.e., overresponsivity and under-) in terms of hypothesized

sensory bases and overt indicators. We identifi ed

assumptions and boundaries of SI theory and SI

intervention.

 What makes occupational therapy practice

different from all other health professions is our

unique emphasis on “doing” occupation. The

term praxis, derived from the Greek, also means

doing, reminding us that, in employing SI theory,

our primary concern is whether the children and

adults with whom we work are able to do what

they need and want to do.

Where Can I Find More?

 Ayres, A. J. (1981; 2005). Sensory integration

and the child. Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services.

18 ■ PART I Theoretical Constructs

 Lane, S. J., Smith Roley, S., & Champagne, T.

(2013). Sensory integration and processing:

Theory and applications to occupational performance. In B. Schell, G. Gillen, & M. Scafa

(Eds.), Willard & Spackman ’ s occupational

therapy (12th ed., pp. 816–868). Philadelphia,

PA: Lippincott, Williams, & Wilkins.

 Parham, L. D., & Mailloux, Z. (2015). Sensory

integration. In J. Case-Smith & J. C. O’Brien

(Eds.), Occupational therapy for children and

adolescents (7th ed., pp. 258–303). St. Louis,

MO: Mosby.

 Smith Roley, S., Schaaf, R. C., & Baltazar Mori,

A. (2019). Ayres Sensory Integration® Frame

of Reference. In P. Kramer, J. Hinojosa, &

T.-H. Howe (Eds.), Frames of Reference for

Pediatric Occupational Therapy (4th Edition,

pp. 87–153). Philadelphia, PA: Wolters

Kluwer.

 Smith Roley, S., Blanche, E. I., & Schaaf, R. C.

(2001). Understanding the nature of sensory

integration with diverse populations. Philadelphia, PA: Harcourt Health Sciences Company.

 References

 American Psychiatric Association . ( 2013 ). Diagnostic

and statistical manual of mental disorders

( 5th ed .). Arlington, VA : American Psychiatric

Publishing.

 Ayres , A. J. ( 1965 ). Patterns of perceptual-motor

dysfunction in children: A factor analytic study .

Perceptual and Motor Skills, 20, 335 – 368 .

 Ayres , A. J. ( 1966a ). Interrelations among perceptualmotor abilities in a group of normal children .

American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 20,

 288 – 292 .

 Ayres , A. J. ( 1966b ). Interrelationships among

perceptual-motor functions in children.

American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 20,

 288 – 292 .

 Ayres , A. J. ( 1969 ). Defi cits in sensory integration

in educationally handicapped children. Journal of

Learning Disabilities, 2, 160 – 168 .

 Ayres , A. J. ( 1972a ). Improving academic scores

through sensory integration. Journal of Learning

Disabilities, 5, 338 – 343 .

 Ayres , A. J. ( 1972b ). Sensory integration and

learning disorders . Los Angeles, CA : Western

Psychological Services.

 Ayres , A. J. ( 1972c ). Southern California Sensory

Integration Tests manual. Los Angeles, CA :

 Western Psychological Services .

 Ayres , A. J. ( 1972d ). Types of sensory integrative

dysfunction among disabled learners.

American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 26,

 13 – 18 .

 Ayres , A. J. ( 1975 ). Southern California Postrotary

Nystagmus Test manual. Los Angeles, CA :

 Western Psychological Services .

 Ayres , A. J. ( 1976 ). The effect of sensory integrative

therapy on learning disabled children: The fi nal

report of a research project . Los Angeles, CA :

 University of Southern California .

 Ayres , A. J. ( 1977 ). Cluster analyses of measures

of sensory integration. American Journal of

Occupational Therapy, 31, 362 – 366 .

 Ayres , A. J. ( 1979 ). Sensory integration and the

child. Los Angeles, CA : Western Psychological

Services.

 Ayres , A. J. ( 1985 ). Developmental dyspraxia and

adult-onset apraxia. Torrance, CA : Sensory

Integration International.

 Ayres , A. J. ( 1989 ). Sensory Integration and

Praxis Tests manual . Los Angeles, CA : Western

Psychological Services.

 Ayres , A. J. , Mailloux , Z. K. , & Wendler , C. L. W.

( 1987 ). Developmental dyspraxia: Is it a unitary

function? Occupational Therapy Journal of

Research, 7, 93 – 110 .

 Ayres , A. J. , & Tickle , L. ( 1980 ). Hyper-responsivity

to touch and vestibular stimulation as a predictor

of responsivity to sensory integrative procedures

in autistic children. American Journal of

Occupational Therapy, 34, 375 – 381 .

 Baranek , G. T. , Foster , L. G. , & Berkson , G. ( 1997 ).

 Tactile defensiveness and stereotyped behaviors .

American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 51,

 91 – 95 .

 Brown , C. , & Dunn , W. ( 2002 ). Adolescent/Adult

Sensory Profi le. San Antonio, TX : Pearson .

 Bulkeley , K. , Bundy , A. , Roberts , J. , & Einfeld , S.

( 2016 ). Family-centered management of sensory

challenges of children with autism: A singlecase experimental design . American Journal

of Occupational Therapy, 70, 7005220040 .

 doi:10.5014/ajot.2016.017822

 DeGangi , G. A. ( 2000 ). Pediatric disorders of

regulation in affect and behavior: A therapist ’ s

guide to assessment and treatment. San Diego,

CA : Academic Press .

 DiJoseph , L. M. ( 1982 ). Independence through

activity: Mind, body, and environment interaction

in therapy . American Journal of Occupational

Therapy, 36, 740 – 744 .

 Dunn , W. ( 1994 ). Performance of typical children

on the sensory profi le . American Journal of

Occupational Therapy, 48, 967 – 974 .

 Dunn , W. ( 2001 ). 2001 Eleanor Clarke Slagle

Lecture—The sensations of everyday

life: Empirical, theoretical, and pragmatic

considerations. American Journal of Occupational

Therapy, 55, 608 – 620 .

 Dunn , W. ( 2014 ). Sensory profi le 2: User ’ s manual.

 San Antonio, TX : Psychological Corporation .

 Dunn , W. , & Brown , C. ( 1997 ). Factor analysis on

the Sensory Profile from a national sample of

children without disabilities. American Journal of

Occupational Therapy, 51, 490 – 495 .

CHAPTER 1 Sensory Integration: A. Jean Ayres’ Theory Revisited ■ 19

 Dunn , W. , & Westman , K. ( 1997 ). The Sensory

Profi le: The performance of a national sample of

children without disabilities. American Journal of

Occupational Therapy, 51, 25 – 34 .

 Fisher , A. G. , & Murray , E. A. ( 1991 ). Introduction

to sensory integration theory . In A. G. Fisher ,

 E. A. Murray , & A. C. Bundy ( Eds .), Sensory

integration: Theory and practice ( pp . 3 – 29 ).

 Philadelphia, PA : F.A. Davis .

 Grandin , T. , & Scariano , M. M. ( 1986 ). Emergence:

Labeled autistic. Novato, CA : Atena .

 Henderson , S. E. , & Sugden , D. A. ( 1992 ). Movement

assessment battery for children manual. London,

UK : Psychological Corporation .

 Keogh , J. , & Sugden , D. ( 1985 ). Movement skill

development. New York, NY : MacMillan .

 Kielhofner , G. , & Fisher , A. G. ( 1991 ). Mind-brain

body relationships. In A. G. Fisher , E. A. Murray ,

& A. C. Bundy ( Eds .), Sensory integration:

Theory and practice ( pp . 30 – 45 ). Philadelphia,

PA : F.A. Davis .

 Lai , J. S. , Fisher , A. G. , Magalhaes , L. C. , & Bundy ,

 A. C. ( 1996 ). Construct validity of the Sensory

Integration and Praxis Tests . Occupational

Therapy Journal of Research, 16, 75 – 97 .

 Lane , A. E. , Young , R. L. , Baker , A. E. Z. , & Angley ,

 M. T. ( 2010 ). Sensory processing subtypes in

autism: Association with adaptive behavior .

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,

40 ( 1 ), 112 – 122 . doi:10.1007/s10803-009-0840-2

 Lane , S. J. , Reynolds , S. , & Dumenci , L. ( 2012 ).

 Sensory over-responsivity and anxiety in typically

developing children and children with autism and

attention defi cit hyperactivity disorder: Cause or

coexistence? American Journal of Occupational

Therapy, 66, 1 – 9 . doi:10.5014/ajot.2012.004523

 Lane , S. J. , Reynolds , S. , & Thacker , L. ( 2010 ).

 Sensory over-responsivity and ADHD:

Differentiating using electrodermal responses,

cortisol, and anxiety . Frontiers in Integrative

Neuroscience, 4 ( March ), 8 . doi:10.3389/

fnint.2010.00008

 Lang , R. , O’Reilly , M. , Healy , O. , Rispoli , M. ,

 Lydon , H. , Streusande , W. , . . . Giesbers , S.

( 2012 ). Sensory integration therapy for autism

spectrum disorders: A systematic review . Research

in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6, 1008 – 1018 .

 Mailloux , Z. , Mulligan , S. , Roley , S. S. , Blanche ,

 E. , Cermak , S. , Coleman , G. G. , . . . Lane , C. J.

( 2011 ). Verifi cation and clarifi cation of patterns

of sensory integrative dysfunction. American

Journal of Occupational Therapy, 65, 143 – 151 .

 doi:10.5014/ajot.2011.000752

 May-Benson , T. ( 2001 ). A theoretical model of

ideation. In E. Blanche , R. Schaaf , & S. Smith

Roley ( Eds .), Understanding the nature of sensory

integration with diverse populations ( pp . 163 – 182 ).

 Tucson, AZ : Therapy Skill Builders (now ProEd) .

 May-Benson , T. A. ( 2005 ). Examining ideational

abilities in children with dyspraxia. Doctoral

dissertation. Boston University. Ann Arbor, MI:

ProQuest.

 May-Benson , T. A. , & Cermak , S. A. ( 2007 ).

 Development of an assessment for ideational

praxis. American Journal of Occupational

Therapy, 61 ( 2 ), 148 – 153 .

 Miller , L. J. , Anzalone , M. E. , Lane , S. J. , Cermak ,

 S. A. , & Osten , E. T. ( 2007 ). Concept evolution

in sensory integration: A proposed nosology for

diagnosis. American Journal of Occupational

Therapy, 61 ( 2 ), 135 – 142 . doi:10.5014/

ajot.61.2.135

 Mulligan , S. ( 1996 ). An analysis of score patterns of

children with attention disorders on the Sensory

Integration and Praxis Tests . American Journal of

Occupational Therapy, 50 ( 8 ), 647 – 654 .

 Mulligan , S. ( 1998 ). Patterns of sensory integration

dysfunction: A confi rmatory factor analysis .

American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 52,

 819 – 828 .

 Mulligan , S. ( 2000 ). Cluster analysis of scores of

children on the Sensory Integration and Praxis

Tests . Occupational Therapy Journal of Research,

20, 256 – 270 .

 Nommensen , A. , & Maas , F. ( 1993 ). Sensory

integration and Down ’ s syndrome . British Journal

of Occupational Therapy, 56, 451 – 454 .

 Parham , L. D. , Ecker , C. , Kuhaneck , H. M. ,

 Henry , D. A. , & Glennon , T. J. ( 2010 ). Sensory

Processing Measure: Manual. Los Angeles, CA :

 Western Psychological Services .

 Parham , L. D. , Roley , S. S. , May-Benson , T. A. ,

 Koomar , J. , Brett-Green , B. , Burke , J. P. , . . .

 Schaaf , R. C. ( 2011 ). Development of a fi delity

measure for research on the effectiveness of the

Ayres Sensory Integration ® intervention . American

Journal of Occupational Therapy, 65, 133 – 142 .

 Pfeiffer , B. A. , Koenig , K. , Kinnealey , M. , Sheppard ,

 M. , & Henderson , L. ( 2011 ). Effectiveness of

sensory integration interventions in children

with autism spectrum disorders: A pilot study .

American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 65,

 76 – 85 .

 Reynolds , S. , Bendixen , R. M. , Lawrence , T. , &

 Lane , S. J. ( 2011 ). A pilot study examining

activity participation, sensory responsiveness,

and competence in children with high functioning

autism spectrum disorder . Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorders, 41 ( 11 ), 1496 – 1506 .

 doi:10.1007/s10803-010-1173-x

 Reynolds , S. , Lane , S. J. , & Richards , L.

( 2010 ). Using animal models of enriched

environments to inform research on sensory

integration intervention for the rehabilitation

of neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of

Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 2 ( 3 ), 120 – 132 .

 Reynolds , S. , Lane , S. J. , & Thacker , L. ( 2011 ).

 Sensory processing, physiological stress, and

sleep behaviors in children with and without

autism spectrum disorders. OTJR: Occupation,

Participation, and Health, 32 ( 1 ), 246 – 257 .

 doi:10.3928/15394492-20110513-02

 Schaaf , R. C. , Benevides , T. , Mailloux , Z. , Faller , P. ,

 Hunt , J. , van Hooydonk , E. , . . . Kelly , D. ( 2013 ).

20 ■ PART I Theoretical Constructs

 An intervention for sensory diffi culties in

children with autism: A randomized trial .

Journal of Autism and Developmental

Disorders , 44 , 1983 – 1988 . doi:10.1007/

s10803-013-1983-8

 Schaaf , R. C. , Hunt , J. , & Benevides , T. ( 2012 ).

 Occupational therapy using sensory integration

to improve participation of a child with autism:

A case report . American Journal of Occupational

Therapy, 66 ( 5 ), 547 – 555 . doi:10.5014/

ajot.2012.004473

 Schmidt , R. A. , & Lee , T. D. ( 2011 ). Motor control

and learning: A behavioral approach ( 5th ed .).

 Champaign, IL : Human Kinetics .

 Sieg , K. W. ( 1988 ). A. Jean Ayres . In B. R. J. Miller ,

 K. W. Sieg , F. M. Ludwig , S. D. Shortridge , & J.

 Van Deusen ( Eds .), Six perspectives on theory for

practice of occupational therapy ( pp . 95 – 142 ).

 Rockville, MD : Aspen .

 White , R. ( 1959 ). Motivation reconsidered: The

concept of competence. Psychological Review, 66,

 297 – 333 .

21

CHAPTER

2

Sensory Integration

in Everyday Life

 L. Diane Parham , PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA ■ Joanna Cosbey , PhD, OTR/L

 Chapter 2

Upon completion of this chapter, the reader will be able to:

✔ Explain how sensory integration (SI) fi ts into a

view of development as multidimensional and

transactional.

✔ Describe the relationship between sensory

processing characteristics and participation in

the everyday occupations of play and leisure;

activities of daily living (ADLs) and instrumental

activities of daily living (IADLs); rest and sleep;

and education and work across the life span.

✔ Describe how assessment and intervention

decisions are infl uenced by the complexity and

transactional nature of development.

 It ’ s not some big event that creates the drama, it ’ s the little

things of everyday life that bring about that drama.

 —Asghar Farhadi

LEARNING OUTCOMES

Purpose and Scope

 The ultimate aim of occupational therapy practice

using sensory integration (SI) principles is to help

children meet the challenges that they encounter

in their everyday lives at home, in school, and at

play. These challenges can be as commonplace as

realizing that one has been gently touched by a

classmate or as unique as inventing a new game

to play with friends on the playground.

 Across the past fi ve decades of research into

SI, much work has focused on identifying patterns of sensory and movement diffi culties and

on documenting outcomes of interventions.

Although the link between SI and what children do in everyday life has been evident since

the early years of Ayres’ work, it would be

useful to cultivate a deeper understanding of the

ways in which SI affects—and is affected by—

participation in the daily activities that structure

and give meaning to children ’ s lives. A deeper

understanding of these connections may lead

to interventions that are more effective because

they are more fi nely tailored to the life situations

of individual children.

 Ayres ( 2004 ) defi ned SI as the “organization

of sensations for use” (p. 5). Those last two

words, “for use,” are revealing. Unlike neuroscientists, who often aim to isolate neural mechanisms, Ayres’ central concern as an occupational

therapist was with how the nervous system organizes sensory information so that the person can

participate in meaningful and in productive occupations. SI-based intervention, therefore, became

a tool for helping children engage in occupations

that lead to rich and meaningful lives.

 The primary purpose of this chapter is to help

readers understand the important relationship

between SI and everyday life—particularly the

performance of everyday occupations, that is,

the activities that people want or need to do. It

also aims to raise questions that may guide future

thinking about this relationship and how knowledge of this relationship might infl uence the

22 ■ PART I Theoretical Constructs

design of programs and environments to benefi t

all people—not just those with clinically identifi ed SI problems.

CASE STUDY ■ NICK

 Nick is an 8-year-old boy assessed as having

sensory integrative diffi culties involving hazy

tactile discrimination and immature visual

perceptual processing. In addition, he has diffi culty with the sequencing and timing aspects

of motor planning. These praxis diffi culties

make it cumbersome for Nick to manage his

body and objects in physical space and in synchrony with time constraints imposed by social

expectations.

 Nick is very aware of the general behavioral expectations placed on him by his teacher

at school. However, his problems with SI and

praxis make it diffi cult for him to comply

with some of these expectations, even though

he wants to do so. For example, he knows he

is supposed to be able to reach into his desk

and fi nd his workbooks and tools (e.g., pencil,

eraser, ruler) quickly, but his desk is chaotic,

and he seems unable to keep it neat for longer

than half a day. His tactile sense is not developed well enough to enable him to fi nd and

handle objects quickly and effi ciently. He needs

to rely on visual inspection to help him fi nd

things in his desk, but his visual system is not

highly skilled either so this becomes an arduous

task. His somatodyspraxia further interferes

with his adeptness in handling classroom materials and tools. His classmates, whose sensory

integrative and praxis abilities are well developed, are able to fi nd and manage their materials quickly and easily, even those who do not

have stellar desk organization habits.

 Closer observation of Nick may reveal that

he actually does not seem aware of some of

the fi ner nuances of his teacher ’ s expectations

for her students. Whereas other students fi gure

out that her expectations require that they work

out strategies for desk management, Nick does

not realize that he needs to do this. This may

be caused at least partially by his sensory and

praxis diffi culties, which have limited his experiences and his success in developing effective

and sophisticated strategies for the organization

and manipulation of objects. In Nick ’ s eyes,

other kids just magically know how to reach

into their desks and instantly fi nd their school

materials. His lack of recognition of the need

for organizational strategies to manage his body

and physical objects in space and time extends

beyond desk management into other realms,

such as playground games and sports with peers.

Thus, he does not realize the full ramifi cations

of many social expectations, such as the expectations for team members during a soccer game.

 Nick ’ s experiences as he engages in a

variety of daily routines—his successes and

failures, pleasures and pains—will infl uence

his future preferences and choices of occupations. He has strong verbal skills and already

shows a defi nite preference for occupations

such as reading and telling jokes. He also has

an avoidance of sports and games that require

skill in sequencing of actions or precision in

object manipulation. Of course, not all of his

choices will be related to his verbal talents or

his sensory integrative diffi culties; many will

be derived from personal experiences with signifi cant people, places, things, and events. For

example, the fl owers that brighten his loving

grandmother ’ s backyard give him feelings of

joy, security, and contentment. The pleasant

memories of his grandmother ’ s garden, as well

as the sensory experiences that he enjoys while

gardening, eventually may lead him to value

the occupation of gardening. Yet, it is possible that his sensory integrative characteristics

will have a lifelong infl uence on his choices

of leisure and work occupations. In an optimal

scenario, he may become a successful attorney

if he has supportive family and friends, if he

continues to hone his verbal talents, and if he

learns to exercise good judgment in deciding

when to use verbal strategies to organize tasks

and when to delegate. He may experience a

great deal of satisfaction and enjoyment while

gardening at home. However, it is very unlikely

that he will become a professional athlete, and

he probably will not choose to participate regularly in a team sport as a leisure activity.

 Clearly SI is a factor in the formation of

Nick ’ s identity, although it most certainly is not

the only factor, and it may not even be the most

important factor. The formation of his identity

is shaped by—and shapes—the occupations

in which he engages. Through time, Nick ’ s

appraisal of who he is becoming in relation

to what he values most in life will affect his

CHAPTER 2 Sensory Integration in Everyday Life ■ 23

overall satisfaction with his life circumstances.

The presence of sensory integrative diffi culties defi nitely does not mean that a person is

doomed to be unsuccessful or unhappy. But

it may present special challenges. Repeated

experiences of failure may cumulatively lead

to feelings of hopelessness and incompetence,

avoidance and fear of challenge, a constricted

range of meaningful occupations, and poor life

satisfaction. Conversely, if a person ’ s experiences lead to a strong sense of self-effi cacy, the

challenges posed by sensory integrative diffi -

culties actually may contribute to the forging of

self-discipline, determination, hope, character,

and, consequently, a rich occupational life with

a high degree of life satisfaction.

The Complexity of Everyday Life

 Occupational therapists have long recognized

that engagement in everyday activities is not

as simple as meets the eye. Getting dressed, for

example, requires the activation and coordination of physical body structures by processes

that involve genomic, physiological, cognitive,

and affective events, all working in synchrony.

Moreover, the specifi c actions and experiences

that occur while getting dressed are shaped by

the constraints and opportunities presented by

the physical environment—materials, objects,

surfaces, and ambient surroundings—as well as

the person ’ s interpretation of the place where

the actions occur and the sociocultural expectations that permeate the situation. Even spiritual

aspects of experience (i.e., the experience of life

meaning and purpose) may be evoked during

commonplace activities, such as dressing. For

example, when diffi culties arise during the performance of ordinary activities, the person may

not only become dissatisfi ed and frustrated but

also may question the worth of his or her own

life. (See also Chapter 1 , Sensory Integration: A.

Jean Ayres’ Theory Revisited.)

 The complexity and multiplicity of factors

that shape lives make it impossible to predict

with any precision what the specifi c life outcomes will be for a particular child who has

sensory integrative problems. The developmental process is extremely complex because,

not only are multiple factors simultaneously

involved, but also it is transactional in nature.

That is, biologically based predispositions and

environmental infl uences mutually affect each

other through dynamic interchanges, so developmental outcomes are the result of a confl uence

of multiple factors that change through time as

they infl uence each other ( Sameroff, 2009 ). This

means that not only is it impossible to predict

long-term life outcomes for an individual child,

but also it is usually inappropriate to look back

across the life of a person who has signifi cant

life problems in search of a single, specifi c cause

of those problems.

 As an example, consider an infant who is

over-responsive to tactile, vestibular, and auditory stimuli and has a young, impulsive mother

who is very stressed by her low-income living

situation and the very limited support she

receives from family or friends. The baby recoils

when touched by his mother, and if environmental sounds become intense (as they often do in

the small apartment with thin walls), he screams

inconsolably. The infant ’ s defensive behaviors

heighten the stress of the mother, who begins to

dread interacting with her infant. She perceives

him as a problem baby who does not like her so

she tries to complete infant caregiving activities

(e.g., feeding, diapering, and bathing) as quickly

as possible in order to be freed to leave him alone

in the bedroom to cry and eventually fall asleep.

Her insensitive handling of him aggravates his

sensory defensiveness, and consequently he cries

increasingly more often and more intensely. In a

worst-case scenario, the mother may try to cope

with this escalating situation by physically and

emotionally neglecting or even abusing her baby.

 Four years go by, and we see the baby grow

into a preschooler who struggles to engage in

play for more than a fl eeting time. Not only does

he have trouble with solitary play but also he

seems unable to play with peers without acting

out aggressively and destructively. In the preschool setting, he stands out as a child who is

having great diffi culty.

 In this example, what was the original cause

of the child ’ s preschool diffi culties? His sensory

defensiveness or his mother ’ s stress? The mother ’ s impulsive personality or the baby ’ s diffi cult

temperament? Is the cause the mother ’ s lack of

access to resources and supports or her lack of

knowledge regarding how to engage with the

child sensitively during daily routines? All are

legitimate causes because each of these issues

24 ■ PART I Theoretical Constructs

affects the others, and all of them interact synergistically to shape the child ’ s life. It would be

inappropriate to single out one as the sole cause,

although some may exert more powerful effects

than others.

 Even in such an extreme example as this one,

the very rocky beginning in the mother-infant

relationship will not necessarily doom this infant

to future failure. (See also Chapter 19 , Application of Sensory Integration with Specifi c Populations.) All along the life course, events and

experiences arise that shape and guide who the

child is becoming. We can imagine a different

preschool outcome for the diffi cult infant in our

previous example if, for example, we introduce

a day-care provider into the picture when the

baby is a toddler. Let us imagine that this daycare provider is gifted at intuiting the toddler ’ s

needs and fi ne-tunes the day-care environment

so that he is not overwhelmed by sensations,

he experiences pleasure and mastery in simple

sensory-motor activities, and he is cared for by

an adult who is nurturing but also sets limits on

his behavior in a consistent manner. Furthermore, the day-care provider forms a positive

relationship with the infant ’ s mother and mentors

her in mothering. It is likely that the child ’ s later

preschool experiences will not be as negative in

this scenario compared with what the situation

might have been had he not had the good fortune

of an optimal day-care experience.

 SI is one of the factors that shapes life outcomes. However, it is only one of many potentially powerful factors. It is probably never

appropriate to identify poor SI as the sole cause

of a child ’ s problems in everyday life or to claim

that a child will have specifi c problems in the

future because of the presence of sensory integrative diffi culties. But SI does contribute to what,

how, and why a person engages in particular

activities at particular times in the life cycle. For

Nick, who was introduced earlier in this chapter,

SI may be a key contributor to his diffi culties

when trying to play sports or manage items in

and around his desk. However, the reactions of

peers and teachers to his disorganization on the

sports fi eld and in the classroom may be just as

powerful, perhaps even more so. For example,

peer ridicule or rejection on the sports fi eld may

instill embarrassment and shame, leading him to

avoid these activities. Avoidance, in turn, results

in fewer opportunities to acquire and master the

motor skills required in these activities, so the

gap in motor skills between Nick and his peers

widens through time.

 So, the answer to the question, “Does poor

sensory integration cause problems in everyday

living?” is “Probably, but it does not determine

outcomes by itself.” Poor SI does not singlehandedly cause diffi culties with daily activities.

It interacts with talents, physical attributes, environmental opportunities, contexts, past experiences, social expectations and responses, and a

host of other factors, all changing through time

and affecting one another, to shape and color

the person ’ s occupational life. (See also the

Spiral Process of Self-Actualization described in

 Chapter 1 , Sensory Integration: A. Jean Ayres’

Theory Revisited.)

 Given the complexity of multiple infl uences

on developmental outcomes, how can we be

sure that SI is a signifi cant factor in daily life,

deserving of attention? Is SI critical enough that

we should pay attention to it when conducting

assessments and planning interventions for children with developmental, learning, or social

diffi culties? The following section examines

research that addresses this issue.

HERE ’ S THE POINT

• Everyday life activities involve ongoing

transactions between complex aspects of

the person—including SI characteristics—and

complex aspects of the activity context.

• SI is one of many factors that shape our

relationships with others and what we are able

to do across the life span.

Sensory Integration and Everyday

Life: The Evidence

 Research exploring the relationship between SI

and participation in everyday life activities is

growing. During the past 10 years, it has become

increasingly clear that SI differences can affect

individuals in all areas of life, starting at birth

and continuing throughout the life span. A systematic review of 35 studies on children with SI

diffi culties indicated that sensory problems were

related to occupational performance diffi culties

in all areas of everyday life: play, activities of

daily living, sleep, and work, including school

CHAPTER 2 Sensory Integration in Everyday Life ■ 25

performance ( Koenig & Rudney, 2010 ). Taken

together, these studies provide evidence that

sensory integrative characteristics are a potentially critical factor in shaping lives, particularly

with respect to people ’ s engagement in occupations. For example, sensory integrative differences may affect how people use their time,

relate to others, and interact with their physical

and social worlds across the life span.

 In this section of the chapter, we will briefl y

examine the relationship between engagement

in occupations across the life span in four major

life areas: (1) play, leisure, and social participation; (2) activities of daily living and instrumental activities of daily living; (3) rest and sleep;

and (4) education and work. Although the majority of the research presented here addresses the

diffi culties and limitations associated with SI

differences, it is important to recognize that it

is possible for there to be some benefi t to these

differences as well. When planning intervention,

the challenge is identifying the combination of

environments, relationships, and activities that

best supports an individual ’ s participation.

 Several specifi c diagnostic groups have been

found to have high rates of SI differences,

such as individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) ( Bagby, Dickie, & Baranek, 2012 ;

 Baranek, David, Poe, Stone, & Watson, 2006 ;

 Hilton, Graver, & LaVesser, 2007 ; Hochhauser

& Engel-Yeger, 2010 ; Lane, Young, Baker, &

Angley, 2010 ; Leekam, Nieto, Libby, Wing, &

Gould, 2007 ; Miller Kuhaneck & Britner, 2013 ;

 Reynolds, Bendixen, Lawrence, & Lane, 2011 );

some genetic conditions, such as Fragile X syndrome (FXS) ( Baranek et al., 2002 ); and developmental coordination disorder (DCD) ( Smyth

& Anderson, 2000 ). However, each of these

diagnoses, by defi nition, involves diffi culties

in areas of functioning, such as communication

skills, motor skills, or intellectual ability, any of

which can interfere with participation in everyday activities. The confounding of SI problems

with developmental and medical diagnostic conditions makes it diffi cult to ascertain whether SI

differences play a signifi cant role in child participation beyond the impact of concomitant diagnostic conditions.

 Few researchers have attempted to extricate the impact of SI factors from other factors

on functional performance. An exception is

research conducted by Reynolds and colleagues

on children with ASD ( Reynolds, Bendixen, et

al., 2011 ). Their multivariate analyses of data for

52 children (26 with ASD and 26 with typical

development) showed that high levels of sensory

over-responsiveness (i.e., sensory sensitivity and

sensory avoidance) were signifi cantly associated with lower levels of competence in activity,

social, and academic participation, suggesting

that sensory diffi culties adversely affect participation regardless of diagnosis. Furthermore, in

these analyses, cognition (as measured by nonverbal IQ) did not signifi cantly contribute to any

aspect of participation.

 In the remainder of this discussion of evidence regarding SI and participation, we will

focus primarily on individuals who are not identifi ed as having a particular diagnostic condition,

although, where appropriate, we refer to similar

fi ndings in children who have diagnosed conditions often characterized by diffi culty processing

sensation (e.g., FXS, ASD, DCD). As you will

see, even individuals who are otherwise typically

developing can experience signifi cant participation diffi culties that are related to their SI abilities. For individuals with diagnosable conditions

involving motor skills or communication, the SI

diffi culties are likely to be even greater. Therefore, by focusing the discussion on individuals

without concomitant developmental or medical

conditions, this chapter aims to present a conservative picture of the impact of SI differences on

everyday life. The reader should keep in mind

that the impact of SI problems is likely to be

compounded by other diagnosable conditions,

when these are present. Conversely, in some situations and tasks, having a sensory difference

may provide an advantage.

Play, Leisure, and Social Participation

 Currently, most of the research that addresses the

impact of SI characteristics on everyday life is

focused on play, leisure, and social participation.

The existing evidence indicates that, even for

individuals without an identifi ed medical diagnosis or disability, sensory integrative characteristics may impact signifi cantly on participation

in play and leisure activities ( Watts, Stagnitti, &

Brown, 2014 ).

 The impacts of sensory integrative differences

can be seen in young infants. An infant ’ s ability

to process and integrate sensory information

26 ■ PART I Theoretical Constructs

effectively can have a clear impact on motherinfant coregulation, which, in turn, directly

affects infant engagement in co-occupations

 ( Esdaile & Olson, 2003 ; Pierce, 2009 ; Zemke

& Clark, 1996 ), such as simple reciprocal social

interactions and play with the caregiver (e.g.,

reciprocal vocalizations and games, such as

peekaboo) ( Fig. 2-1 ). Researchers have found

that infants with SI differences may be fussier and

have more diffi culty forming typical attachments

than infants with developmentally appropriate SI

( DeSantis, Coster, Bigsby, & Lester, 2004 ; Hofer,

 2006 ; Purvis, McKenzie, Cross, & Razuri, 2013 ).

These challenges affect the quality of engagement as well as the amount of time that infants

can sustain engagement in play and social participation and can be magnifi ed when the mother

as well as the infant has diffi culties integrating

sensory information effectively ( Turner, Cohn, &

Koomar, 2012 )—a refl ection of the transactional

nature of the developmental process.

 Researchers have found that early diffi culties

connecting with and relating to other people can

persist throughout childhood. In early childhood,

SI differences appear to be related to play in

children with ASD. Specifi cally, a child ’ s praxis

skills and abilities to process visual, touch, proprioceptive, and vestibular information are associated with social play (e.g., sharing or playing

cooperatively) ( Miller Kuhaneck & Britner,

 2013 ). Additionally, sensory integrative characteristics are related to the types of toys children

select for play in early childhood. A study of

typically developing 3- to 5-year-olds showed

that children who demonstrate a preference for

toys that promote fantasy play (e.g., dollhouses,

toy dinosaurs) had a tendency to exhibit more

sensation-seeking behaviors than peers who

preferred creative art and building materials

( Mische Lawson & Dunn, 2008 ). Further evidence indicates that sensory over-responsiveness

of parents is signifi cantly associated with overresponsiveness in their otherwise typically developing children and that parent tendency to not

engage in sensory-seeking activities may limit

child exposure to sensory-stimulating activities

( Welters-Davis & Mische Lawson, 2011 ). These

fi ndings, again, suggest that SI may play an

important role in the transactional processes of

child development.

 The impact of SI on activity choices and play

continues through middle childhood. Many children with sensory integrative diffi culties may be

able to compensate well for their sensory differences by harnessing strengths to develop typical

play skills ( Bundy, 1989 ; Clifford & Bundy,

 1989 ). However, recent studies indicate that

sensory differences often interfere with play participation ( Fig. 2-2 ). Bundy, Shia, Qi, and Miller

 ( 2007 ) found that sensory modulation dysfunction has a negative impact on the playfulness

of children. A case study presented by Benson,

Nicka, and Stern ( 2006 ) illustrates how this may

be manifested. They described the play characteristics of a 6-year-old boy with sensory integrative dysfunction who demonstrated limited and

cautious play, poor attention to play activities,

and minimal emotional investment in his play.

 In middle childhood, SI diffi culties may lead

to limited play preferences and restricted social

networks with peers. Cosbey, Johnston, Dunn,

and Bauman ( 2012 ) found that children with SI

differences engaged in more solitary play than

their peers and did not show the same shift to

organized games-with-rules play as their peers.

Similar fi ndings have been discovered for children with DCD ( Smyth & Anderson, 2000 ).

Additionally, through structured interviews of

children with and without SI differences, Cosbey,

Johnston, and Dunn ( 2010 ) found that children

with SI differences tended to demonstrate more

limited social networks, and spent less time

FIGURE 2-1 Sensory integrative abilities in both the

mother and child are important for bonding as well

as the development of the infant ’ s social and selfregulatory abilities.

CHAPTER 2 Sensory Integration in Everyday Life ■ 27

with friends, than peers without SI diffi culties.

Although the children ’ s activity preferences were

similar across the two groups, children with SI

differences indicated the least enjoyment for

activities with formal rules and clear expectations. Interestingly, three of the most preferred

activities for children with SI differences (quiet

tabletop activities, pretend play, and computer

or video games) were the three areas that were

the least preferred by their peers. Hochhauser

and Engel-Yeger ( 2010 ) reported similar fi ndings

for children with ASD, indicating that the children with ASD had limited ranges of activities

and tended to have a smaller social network than

their typically developing peers.

 Other researchers examining the play, leisure,

and social participation of children with ASD

have found that there is a link between social

competence and sensory processing abilities,

with the presence of more sensory diffi culties

related to poorer social competence ( Hilton et al.,

 2007 ; Reynolds, Bendixen, et al., 2011 ). Further,

sensory integrative abilities impact family activity choices of families of children with and

without ASD ( Bagby et al., 2012 ). Collectively,

this body of research suggests that a school-aged

child ’ s play, leisure, and social participation may

be impacted by SI differences. The impact seems

to be even more pronounced when the sensory

integrative differences are concomitant with

other conditions, such as ASD.

 Limited research examines the relationship

between SI and everyday life for adolescents.

Existing studies indicate that risk-taking and

sensation-seeking behaviors are characteristic

of typical adolescents (e.g., Lightfoot, 1997 ;

 Zuckerman, 1994 ). Zuckerman ( 1994 ) defi ned

sensation seeking as “the seeking of varied,

novel, complex, and intense sensations and experiences, and the willingness to take physical,

social, legal, and fi nancial risks for the sake of

such experience” (p. 27). Some evidence (e.g.,

 Greene, Krcmar, Walters, Rubin, & Hale, 2000 )

suggests that adolescents who engage in more

sensation-seeking behaviors (as defi ned by Zuckerman) are more likely to engage in delinquent

behaviors. These teens seem to have intense

responses to novel stimuli and seek out activities

and opportunities to experience these stimuli.

However, other researchers ( Shea & Wu, 2012 )

have found that adolescents who are involved

with the juvenile justice system tend to show

fewer sensation-seeking behaviors and more

sensation-avoiding behaviors than other teens.

 These two sets of fi ndings appear to contradict each other, but this may be because of

the use of different theoretical models by the

researchers. The term sensation seeking connotes different meanings depending on the theoretical framework being used. In contrast with

the Zuckerman conceptualization, Shea and Wu

 ( 2012 ) used this term in a manner that is consistent with Dunn ’ s conceptualization of sensory

seeking as a manifestation of high neurological

threshold for responses to sensory stimulation

(i.e., requiring intense sensory input in order to

respond to stimuli) and in combination with a

tendency to actively seek out input to reach that

threshold ( Dunn, 1997 ). Shea and Wu ( 2012 )

also used Dunn ’ s conceptualization of sensation

avoiding (i.e., active avoidance of sensations in

conjunction with a low neurological threshold

for responses to sensory stimulation). Regardless

FIGURE 2-2 Children with sensory-based dyspraxia

may have diffi culty learning new motor skills and get

left out of social activities and experiences typically

engaged in by their peers.

28 ■ PART I Theoretical Constructs

of whether the Zuckerman or Dunn conceptualization of “sensation seeking” was used, the

researchers found that adolescents who have

intense reactions to sensory input (either actively

seeking or actively avoiding) are more likely

to engage in delinquent behaviors. What is not

clear from the current research is how active

sensory-seeking and avoiding behaviors may be

related to delinquent behavior.

 Other researchers have found that teens who

have been involved negatively with law enforcement tend to have SI diffi culties as demonstrated

by their performance on vestibular- and praxisrelated tests ( Fanchiang, Snyder, Zobel-Lachiusa,

Loeffl er, & Thompson, 1990 ). In another study,

sensory integrative differences were found to be

related to non-suicidal self-injurious behaviors

(e.g., self-cutting) in young adolescents ( Christensen, 2012 ). Teens engaging in these behaviors

demonstrated greater sensitivity to sounds as

well as tendencies toward sensation avoiding and

low registration. Although this body of research

does not directly address conventionally accepted

modes of play, leisure, and social participation,

it provides some insight into some behaviors

in which teens with SI differences may engage

outside of time designated for school, work, selfcare, and sleep.

 SI differences may indirectly affect engagement in play, leisure, and social participation

in adulthood through its impact on emotional

states and social relationships. SI differences are

strongly associated with anxiety ( Engel-Yeger

& Dunn, 2011b ; Green & Ben-Sasson, 2010 ;

 Kinnealey, Koenig, & Smith, 2011 ; Reynolds &

Lane, 2009 ), and a growing body of evidence

on adults suggests a relationship between SI and

affect ( Engel-Yeger & Dunn, 2011a ); relationship styles and ability to cope ( Jerome & Liss,

 2005 ); and temperament, including expression

of anger ( Stols, van Heerden, van Jaarsveld, &

Nel, 2013 ), social introversion, depression, and

HERE ’ S THE EVIDENCE

Reynolds, S., Bendixen, R. M., Lawrence, T., & Lane,

S. J. (2011). A pilot study examining activity participation, sensory responsiveness, and competence

in children with high functioning autism spectrum

disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41, 1496–1506.

This descriptive cross-sectional study compared

activity participation and perceived competence in

two groups of children with ( n = 27) and without

( n = 28) ASD from 6 to 12 years of age. The Child

Behavior Checklist (CBCL) competence scales were

completed by parents who answered questions

related to their child ’ s participation in the areas of

activities, social life, and school performance. The

Sensory Profi le also was completed by parents as a

means of comparing children ’ s sensory processing

abilities to their competence in performing and participating in daily life tasks. When the researchers

examined the types of leisure activities participated

in by each group, the children with ASD had more

involvement in video games, playing with transportation vehicles, and reading books; they had

less involvement in dramatic play, play with dolls

or action fi gures, or in arts and crafts activities.

Overall, parents of kids who were developing typically were able to identify more leisure activities

participated in by their children than parents of kids

with ASD. Children with ASD were also found to

participate in fewer jobs or chores at home, with

27% of children with ASD having no chores at

home compared with only 7.6% of kids developing

typically who had fewer than two chores. Overall,

children with ASD had less involvement in chores

related to animal care, babysitting, or general

cleaning around the home. When the researchers

compared competence scores between the two

groups, the group with ASD was found to have signifi cantly lower levels of competence in each area

( p = .000 across all three domains: activity participation, school performance, and social participation). Subsequent analyses identifi ed that sensory

over-responsive scores on the Sensory Profi le (elevated behaviors of sensation avoiding and sensory

sensitivity) were signifi cantly correlated with lower

levels of competence. The authors suggest in their

discussion that children who show sensory overresponsiveness may be less likely to engage in activities that require the processing of self-perceived

noxious sensory inputs and that they may not

perform tasks successfully when they do attempt to

engage. This study suggests a role for occupational

therapists in helping children with sensory modulation disorders fi nd and be successful in meaningful

play and leisure activities as well as helping parents

to determine which jobs or chores the child can

take on within the household to ensure he or she

experiences the same types of learning and maturational opportunities as other children.

CHAPTER 2 Sensory Integration in Everyday Life ■ 29

impulse control ( Kimball, Birstler, Bosse, Nelson,

& Woods, 2012 ). Further, research ( Kinnealey

et al., 2011 ) suggests that adults with SI differences may have more limited social supports

and lower perceived health-related quality of life,

including social functioning. Finally, quantitative research as well as fi rst-person accounts of

living with SI differences ( Kinnealey, Oliver, &

Wilbarger, 1995 ; McCarter, 2010 ) suggest that

sensory integrative differences may impact

adults’ interpersonal relationships via physical

intimacy and activity choices.

 This rapidly growing research base provides

evidence that SI differences are related to occupations (e.g., play, leisure) and performance,

especially social interaction, skills. Throughout

the life span, SI has the potential to infl uence our

occupation and activity choices, from the types

of toys a toddler plays with to the leisure activities of adolescents and engagement in social

activities by adults. Additionally, SI differences

are connected to diffi culties in fundamental

social-interaction skills that affect engagement

in play and social participation, from fussiness

in an infant to anger and anxiety in an adult.

However, because of the transactional nature of

the developmental process, negative outcomes

for individuals with sensory integrative differences are not inevitable. Supportive contexts and

environments that permit an individual to capitalize on strengths and talents (i.e., client factors)

while building new performance skills may lead

to a highly successful and satisfying life.

Activities of Daily Living and

Instrumental Activities of Daily Living

 Engagement in activities of daily living (ADLs),

such as dressing, eating, and bathing, as well as

instrumental activities of daily living (IADLs),

such as shopping, preparing food, and housekeeping, are essential and often time-consuming

aspects of everyday life. As with play, leisure,

and social interaction skills, evidence shows

that SI differences impact performance of ADLs

and IADLs beginning in infancy. Infants with

SI differences tend to be fussier and have more

diffi culty establishing appropriate attachment to

caregivers (e.g., DeSantis et al., 2004 ; Hofer,

 2006 ; Purvis et al., 2013 ), which can affect an

infant ’ s ability to participate in co-occupations

with caregivers, such as dressing, bathing, and

eating. Sensory defensiveness may have a negative effect on breastfeeding ( Radzyminski, 2005 ;

 Weiss-Salinas & Williams, 2001 ).

 Eating diffi culties appear to persist through

childhood and adolescence. One of the most

widely documented ADL areas impacted by SI

differences is the area of feeding and mealtimes.

Children with sensory integrative differences tend

to have inordinate diffi culty accepting new foods

( Blissett & Fogel, 2013 ), and many children

referred to feeding clinics tend to have sensory

integrative differences ( Davis et al., 2013 ). They

also may have diffi culty participating in family

routines related to meal preparation because of

the sensory characteristics of those routines, such

as loud appliances and the tactile and olfactory

aspects of cooking ( Bagby et al., 2012 ). Sensory

integrative diffi culties also have been implicated

in other areas of children ’ s ADLs, such as toileting and dressing ( Bellefeuille, Schaaf, &

Polo, 2013 ; O’Neil, 2010 ; Schaaf, 2011 ). Reynolds and Lane ( 2008 ) presented case reports of

children with tactile sensitivity, illustrating that

ADLs, including diffi culties with hair washing

and combing, nail clipping, toothbrushing, dressing, and eating, can be diffi cult for them. These

diffi culties may be compounded by other conditions. For example, Baranek and colleagues

 ( 2002 ) reported that boys with FXS who had

increased aversive and avoidance reactions to

sensory stimulation were also more likely to be

less independent with their ADLs.

 Sensory integrative diffi culties are related

to ADL and IADL performance by adults. In a

qualitative study, Kinnealey, Oliver, and Wilbarger ( 1995 ) documented that adults with

sensory integrative differences reported restrictions in clothing and food choices, diffi culty

with meal preparation activities, and diffi culty

with visits to the dentist. The adults in this study

also reported that their sensory integrative differences impacted other aspects of self-care, such as

choice of jewelry and whether or not they wore

make-up. Pohl, Dunn, and Brown ( 2003 ) found

that adults tend to demonstrate lower levels of

registration of sensory input as they age, which

has potential negative implications for their

ability to drive, navigate within their community,

and complete self-care activities appropriately.

 This research indicates that sensory integrative differences have a lifelong impact on an

individual ’ s participation in ADLs and IADLs.

30 ■ PART I Theoretical Constructs

Feeding, the earliest and most pervasive ADL,

appears to be related to sensory integrative abilities from birth through adulthood and may affect

nutritional intake as well as ability to participate

in meals shared with others. As the performance

demands increase, individuals with sensory

integrative diffi culties are increasingly likely to

encounter ADLs and IADLs that are stressful.

The presence of sensory diffi culties, therefore,

may restrict engagement in a wide range of

activities and, ultimately, may limit the person ’ s

access to community places and events.

Rest and Sleep

 Little research has examined the relationship

between SI differences and the vitally important

occupations of rest and sleep, but the existing

evidence is consistent in fi nding a signifi cant

association between sensory differences and

sleep diffi culties. Researchers ( Wiener, Long,

DeGangi, & Battaile, 1996 ) have found that

full-term infants who demonstrated SI differences at 7 to 18 months of age were likely to

have diffi culties with sleep ( Fig. 2-3 ). Other

researchers have documented a link between

sensory hypersensitivity and disrupted sleep

among children without disabilities ( Shochat,

Tzischinsky, & Engel-Yeger, 2009 ) as well as

adults ( Engel-Yeger & Shochat, 2012 ). At least

one study ( Reynolds, Lane, & Thacker, 2011 )

has documented that children with ASD have a

high prevalence of atypical sensory behaviors

that are associated with sleep disturbance. Furthermore, in this study, behavioral and physiological (i.e., body function) measures of sensory

responsivity distinguished between good and

poor sleepers with 85% accuracy, suggesting

that sensory diffi culties are important factors in

understanding and planning intervention for the

many children with ASD who have sleep defi cits.

Further research is necessary to fully describe the

relationship between rest and sleep and SI patterns, but this preliminary research suggests that

there is a potentially powerful link that should be

explored more completely.

Education and Work

 Although little research has specifi cally assessed

the relationship between SI and education or

work performance, many researchers have

theorized about the potential relationship. For

example, Geva and Feldman ( 2008 ) presented

a conceptual framework of infant development

that places the ability to regulate and integrate

sensory information as a key element of development. According to their model, secondary skills

of emotion and attention regulation, which ultimately lead to higher level learning skills (e.g.,

cognitive processing and self-regulation), are

dependent on SI. There are several other models

that also identify SI as an essential foundational

element of learning experiences, which could

include participation in education and work

activities ( Dunn, 1997 ; Trott, Laurel, & Windeck,

 1993 ; Williams & Shellenberger, 1996 ).

 The relationship between SI and education

has been explored in school-aged children and

adolescents. SI differences have been found to be

related to academic achievement in both reading

and math ( Parham, 1998 ). Specifi cally, sensory

integrative differences in children who were

6 to 8 years old were found to predict arithmetic achievement 4 years later, after statistically

FIGURE 2-3 Sensory sensitivities may make it more

diffi cult for children to fall asleep and stay asleep.

CHAPTER 2 Sensory Integration in Everyday Life ■ 31

controlling for intellectual ability and family

socioeconomic level. This relationship was

strong, particularly with regard to praxis skills.

A different relationship was found between SI

and reading achievement, with SI demonstrating a predictive relationship at the older (but not

younger) ages ( Parham, 1998 ). A similar fi nding

was reported for children with FXS ( Baranek et

al., 2002 ), with lower scores in school functioning linked to greater SI differences. Diffi culties

with participation in typical school activities,

including navigating the school environment,

recess, assemblies, lunch, handwriting, and participating in other classroom activities, also have

been reported ( Bagby et al., 2012 ; McCarter,

 2010 ; Reynolds & Lane, 2008 ; Smyth & Anderson, 2000 ).

 Although the relationship between SI and education or work has not been thoroughly explored

during adulthood, our understanding of SI and

the critical role it plays in supporting participation in life activities suggests that it is likely that

a relationship exists ( Fig. 2-4 ). Some fi rst-person

narratives and case reports support this hypothesis. For example, Fanchiang ( 1996 ) presented a

case report of a young man with a learning disability and SI differences. Fanchiang theorized

that the young man ’ s career choices may have

been infl uenced by his SI characteristics, with

the role of massage therapist fulfi lling his own

needs for intense sensory input. Temple Grandin,

a well-known adult with ASD, has written about

the relationship between vocational choices and

sensory integrative as well as cognitive characteristics and interests ( Grandin, 2006 ), and her

own highly successful career in designing equipment for the cattle industry clearly capitalizes on

her visual strengths. Therefore, it appears that

SI characteristics infl uence career choices and

that building on strengths while acknowledging

sensory-based challenges may lead to a more

successful and satisfying work life.

HERE ’ S THE POINT

• Research evidence indicates that SI

characteristics infl uence a person ’ s participation

in various occupations—play and leisure, ADLs,

sleep and rest, and work and education—

across the life span.

• Limitations in participation, attributable to

differences in sensory integrative processing,

may be seen in diagnostic populations typically

seen by occupational therapists, including

adults and children with ASD, DCD, FXS, and

sensory integrative disorders.

Implications for Assessment

and Intervention

 In this chapter, we have focused on how SI infl uences participation in everyday occupations. So

far, we have discussed the transactional process

of development, which infl uences how SI interacts with other factors to affect participation, and

we have summarized the research evidence surrounding the infl uence of SI on engagement in

everyday occupations. Now we will consider a

few of the implications that these ideas have for

clinical practice.

FIGURE 2-4 An individual with sensory overresponsivity may choose to pursue work activities

that require limited social interaction and occur in a

controlled, low stimulation environment.

32 ■ PART I Theoretical Constructs

Assessment: Looking to the Future,

Considering the Past

 Because occupational therapy is a practice profession that is centrally concerned with occupation as a key aspect of health and well-being,

therapists should place occupation at the front

and center of the assessment process. But

because occupation is complex and affected by

multiple ongoing transactions between person

and context, the assessment process must be

fl exible and responsive to the unique situation of

each child and family.

 Periodically, leaders in different areas of

occupational therapy practice (e.g., Brown,

 2012 ) have recommended that therapists avoid a

bottom-up assessment approach, instead embracing a top-down approach. By “bottom-up,” we

mean that assessment begins with a focus on specifi c components believed to be affected by the

child ’ s condition, such as strength, coordination,

or perceptual skills ( Trombly, 1993 ). In the fi eld

of SI, an example of a bottom-up assessment

strategy might be administration of the Sensory

Integration and Praxis Test (SIPT) ( Ayres, 1989 )

as a fi rst step in assessment of a child with a

diagnosis of attention defi cit disorder. If this

is the initial approach to assessment, we might

consider it to be component driven ( Gray, 1998 )

because concerns with sensory, perceptual, and

motor components are driving the assessment

and subsequent intervention.

 In contrast, “top-down” refers to an assessment process that begins with informationgathering related to occupation; in other words,

what the person wants and needs to do, the contexts for performing these occupations, and the

current strengths and limitations in performing

the desired occupations ( Brown, 2012 ; Coster,

 1998 ; Fisher, 1998 ; Fisher & Marterella, 2019 ;

 Fisher & Short-DeGraff, 1993 ). For example,

an occupational therapist who uses a top-down

strategy with a child diagnosed with attention

defi cit disorder initially will gather information regarding what the child wants and needs

to do (according to the child, the parents, and

other important people in the child ’ s life, such

as teachers), what the contexts are for the child ’ s

current occupations (e.g., school, home, community settings), and what the child and others

perceive as the current successes and problems

the child experiences in doing valued occupations. This initial investigation then will lead to

decisions regarding whether further assessment

of specifi c skills or abilities is warranted, including administration of the SIPT.

 Use of a top-down strategy, therefore, puts

the assessment of sensory integrative functioning into the broader context of the child ’ s life.

This contextualizing of SI assessment is potentially more likely to be perceived by families as

helpful and relevant, compared with assessment

procedures that seem remote from everyday life.

 A problem with the top-down assessment

approach is that it focuses on problems in occupation only at the time of assessment ( Coster, 1998 ;

 Fisher, 1998 ). However, occupational therapists

project into the future as they plan the course of

therapy with their patients. In fact, in their qualitative study of occupational therapists engaged in

practice, Mattingly and Fleming ( 1994 ) claimed

that occupational therapists imagine who their

patients will become in the future and, further,

that they create stories with the patient about

what has happened in the past and what will

happen in the future as the person ’ s life unfolds.

Perhaps occupational therapists should include in

their clinical assessments the hopes and wishes

of the family for the future, especially with

regard to the occupations that the child will want

and need to do in a few years (for example, see

 Cohn, Kramer, Schub, & May-Benson, 2014 ).

 The transactional view of development also

suggests that assessment should be future oriented.

Because development is continually shaped and

channeled by transactions between the child and

the environment, assessment should be repeated

through time. This could take place during the

ongoing process of intervention or it could be in

the form of intermittent monitoring. The purpose

of reassessment is to ensure that the child ’ s occupational development is moving in the desired

direction, to detect when new issues related to

the child ’ s occupations have emerged, and to

re-evaluate intervention or occupational options

in order to reformulate the most benefi cial plan

for helping the child and family, given changes

that have occurred. Note that this assessment

strategy continues to use a top-down approach,

with occupation as the primary reference point,

but assessment is not conducted only at one specifi c point in time. Instead, assessment is ongoing

or recurs intermittently as time moves forward.

 An implication of bringing a future orientation into clinical assessment is that prevention of

CHAPTER 2 Sensory Integration in Everyday Life ■ 33

potential problems comes to the forefront. When

assessment addresses only concerns related to

the present, one ’ s attention is not drawn systematically to information that may signal risk

for future problems. A present-only assessment

strategy may lead to concluding that a particular

child or family will not benefi t from intervention

because there is limited evidence of an occupational problem at the current time, when, in fact,

some assistance or guidance at the present time

may avert problems that are likely to emerge in

the future. If one is consciously imagining the

future occupational life of a child by extrapolating from what is known about present occupational patterns and contexts, as well as the

current status of performance components,

including SI, it is more likely that risk factors for

later occupational diffi culties will be identifi ed

and that something will be done to minimize or

counteract the risk. For example, Kyle, the child

featured in Chapter 11 (Interpreting and Explaining Evaluation Data), experienced success in

kindergarten, despite sensory integrative dysfunction, because his teacher accommodated so

well for his diffi culties. However, in evaluation,

his family and therapist sought to offset the diffi -

culties they anticipated Kyle having in fi rst grade

with a different teacher and when classroom

demands increased.

 Granted, not much is known about factors

that predict future problems with a satisfying,

productive, and enjoyable work and play life.

Additionally, health-care and education systems

in much of the world are not prepared to designate funding for large-scale preventive programs. Nevertheless, preventive efforts may

be economically advantageous in the long run.

Perhaps future research will identify powerful early predictors of occupational problems

in childhood, adolescence, and adulthood. We

may fi nd that there are times when screening or

assessment of functions, such as SI, is appropriate for preventive purposes, as when children

are screened for vision or hearing problems so

that intervention can be introduced before such

problems have adverse effects on school performance ( Fig. 2-5 ). For example, Thomas and colleagues ( 2015 ) found preliminary evidence that

they could predict the development of sensory

over-responsiveness in childhood using the presence of feeding and sleep diffi culties of young

infants as a proxy. They hypothesized that early

identifi cation and intervention could minimize

the impact of the sensory diffi culties on attachment, emotion regulation, and, later, engagement

in play, self-care, sleep, and school participation.

 Possibly intervention aimed at minimizing the

impact of sensory diffi culties at a young age may

PRACTICE WISDOM

Recently adding part-time clinical practice in an

outpatient occupational therapy clinic to my

daily activities, I found that each of the four children I was seeing had some features of sensory

integrative disorder, including problems with

sensory modulation, discrimination, and praxis.

For each child, performance of functional skills

and participation was impacted by these sensory

integrative problems. Some kids had issues with

toileting, hair brushing, or sequencing their

nighttime hygiene routines, whereas others

had diffi culty participating with peers in agelevel games and sports. However, the goals that

had been written previously for each child were

focused neither on function nor participation.

There were goals for demonstrating in-hand

manipulation skills but not for being able to open

milk containers at lunch. There were goals for

being able to sequence an obstacle course but

not goals for being able to sequence the steps

needed to brush teeth. And there were goals

for improving self-regulation but no mention

for how self-regulatory skills would transfer over

into being able to play a board game with a

friend. I found, in talking to parents, that what

they were really hoping their child got out of

occupational therapy was much different from

established goals. I also found that the kids had

expressed goals that weren ’ t refl ected in the

evaluations or therapy goals—one adolescent

noting, “I would just like to be able to make

a real friend.” In modifying treatment goals to

be more function- and participation-focused, I

found that I got better buy-in overall from the

children and their parents. I enjoyed seeing how

working on the children ’ s underlying sensory

issues in an outpatient setting could translate

into meaningful gains in occupational performance in home, school, and community settings. Take home message: Meaningful goals

focus on what children and parents want within

the context of their real lives. Theory helps us

develop interventions and predict change, but

the needs of families and children always take

priority over theoretical constructs.

34 ■ PART I Theoretical Constructs

maximize children ’ s later occupational participation. Ultimately, this use of performance components for anticipating diffi culties is directed

toward enhancing the future occupational life

of the child; therefore, we can think of it as a

top-down approach in which the top is projected

into the future. Even though research is limited

and we must keep in mind the limitations of the

existing research, occupational therapists can

incorporate estimations of risk into their assessments by imagining where the child and family

might end up in the future if they continue on

their current course.

 In considering how to minimize child and

family risk, occupational therapists need to consider all potential resources available to the child

or family. For example, we have evidence ( Reynolds, Lane, & Thacker, 2011 ) that sensory modulation diffi culties in early childhood are strongly

associated with sleep disorders, particularly in

children with ASD. Occupational therapists can

contribute to intervention programs for children

with ASD by sharing this information with families

whose children with ASD have sensory overresponsiveness and helping them to develop

family strategies that promote healthy sleep patterns by creating soothing environments and predictable bedtime routines. We also have evidence

( Parham, 1998 ) that praxis diffi culties in early

childhood place a child at risk for later academic

problems, especially in mathematics. Occupational therapists working with young children

with dyspraxia may want to share this information

with parents and help them plan ways to give the

child additional support for academic skill development before the child experiences repeated

failure. Referrals to social workers, psychologists,

other professionals, family and child-care service

providers, and community children ’ s programs

also can be important for connecting families

with resources that can help minimize risk.

Consideration of Intervention Options

 The multidimensionality of occupation and its

enmeshment with environmental contexts suggests that many options for intervention should

be considered if clinical assessment indicates

that a child is experiencing diffi culty with occupations. The occupational therapist ’ s challenge is

to identify variables that can be altered in order

to affect a positive change in the entire system

comprising a child ’ s transactions with his or her

environment and to envision how a process of

change might unfold through time. Then intervention options are selected and orchestrated to

channel the child ’ s and family ’ s occupations in a

direction that will move them toward achieving

the desired occupational goals.

 When sensory integrative diffi culties contribute to a child ’ s occupational problems, the

occupational therapist must consider whether

intervention should focus on altering the child ’ s

ability to participate in occupations, on changing

the child ’ s experience by altering aspects of the

environment to support the child ’ s engagement,

or on some combination of these strategies. If

improvement in sensory integrative abilities is

desirable, then individual occupational therapy

using an Ayres SI approach should be given the

fi rst consideration, as evidence of its effectiveness

is supported by several well-designed randomized clinical trials ( Miller, Coll, & Schoen, 2007 ;

 Pfeiffer, Koenig, Kinnealey, Sheppard, & Henderson, 2011 ; Schaaf et al., 2014 ). Alternatively,

FIGURE 2-5 Children with praxis problems in early

childhood are at risk for academic problems later on.

Image courtesy Thinkstock/jupiterimages.

CHAPTER 2 Sensory Integration in Everyday Life ■ 35

or in conjunction with individual therapy, it may

be helpful to encourage the family to advocate for

school-based group programs (e.g., Hartshorn et

al., 2001 ; Koenig, Buckley-Reen, & Garg, 2012 )

or enroll the child in enrichment programs that are

available in the community, such as gym classes

or swimming. If the child has signifi cant sensory

modulation issues, perhaps a program that helps

the child develop strategies to cope better with

everyday routines and environments (e.g., the

Alert Program; Williams & Shellenberger, 1996 ;

see also Chapter 20 , Planning and Implementing

Intervention Using Sensory Integration Theory)

would be benefi cial. If specifi c motor or social

skills are key problems, then skill training, either

individually or in a group, might be appropriate.

Is immediate success in troublesome occupations

an urgent need? If so, consultation or coaching

to suggest changes in tasks or activities and

their environmental contexts may be the priority

(see also Chapter 17 , Using Sensory Integration

Theory in Coaching). Dunbar ( 1999 ) and Bundy

and Green (see Chapter 22 , Viewing Intervention

Through Different Lenses) provided relevant

case examples. Alternatively, occupational therapists may collaborate with families to organize

their lifestyles so that sensory needs and interests of children are integrated into daily family

routines at the same time that parental needs,

interests, occupational styles, and values are

accommodated ( Dunn, 2014 ; Dunn, Cox, Foster,

Mische-Lawson, & Tanquary, 2012 ).

 Consideration of family and child resources,

preferences, and occupational styles, as well as

community resources, is critical in recommending and discussing intervention options with families. When considering how to be most helpful

to a child and family, it is useful to imagine the

transactional process of development projecting into the future. Who is this child becoming? Where are the lives of the child and family

headed? What resources are available to them?

How supportive is the environment? How might

the trajectory be altered if various intervention

options are introduced?

HERE ’ S THE POINT

• Assessment using the SI approach should take

into account the complex, transactional nature

of participation in everyday activities, including

strengths and resources available.

• SI approaches should address goals that refl ect

the values and needs of the child and family,

are focused on function and participation, and

refl ect outcomes that are evidence-based.

Summary and Conclusions

 From its inception, SI theory has viewed the child

as an active agent in the world, whose engagement with the environment affects the development of competence and satisfaction in doing

occupations. Within this theory, the neurobiological construct of SI holds an important position

as a mediator between the child ’ s physical self

and the external world. Because the neural processes and behavioral expressions of SI shape

the person ’ s capacity and willingness to act on

the environment, SI is relevant to the construction of the self through the doing of occupations.

 However, SI is only one of many factors that

infl uence occupation; it interplays with social

expectations, physical environments, and personal experiences in shaping an individual ’ s

occupational life. (See also Chapter 1 , Sensory

Integration: A. Jean Ayres’ Theory Revisited.)

We have evidence that sensory integrative characteristics infl uence the person ’ s competencies

in doing various activities as well as personal

choices of occupations and how to perform them,

throughout the life span.

 The reciprocal relationship between SI and

occupation opens the door to a multitude of intervention possibilities. Because SI affects engagement in occupations, it is one of the many factors

that may be considered in assessing the reasons

for why a child may be experiencing diffi culties with occupations, such as participating in

home, school, and play activities. One valuable,

evidence-based course of action for intervention

is to strengthen sensory integrative capacities

through provision of individual Ayres Sensory

Integration ® (ASI) intervention. An additional

option is to use knowledge of a child ’ s SI strengths

and limitations to modify the tasks, routines, and

environments of the child ’ s occupational life in

order to maximize the child ’ s success and family

satisfaction in the immediate contexts of daily

life. Several researchers (e.g., Bulkeley, Bundy,

Roberts, & Einfeld, 2016 ; Dunn et al., 2012 ;

 Kientz & Dunn, 2012 ) have had some degree of

success with context-based interventions. Future

36 ■ PART I Theoretical Constructs

research also may assist us in evaluating the

extent to which sensory integrative abilities can

be developed through community-based activity

programs, such as classes in creative movement,

yoga, swimming, or other physical activities.

Research remains to be done to clarify when particular approaches are most benefi cial, for whom,

and under which circumstances.

 SI is not simply a neurological process contained entirely within the individual. It is a

complex process through which the nervous

system mediates transactions between individuals and the world. In this view, SI serves as a

scaffold for human agency and, therefore, is

linked inextricably with occupation.

Where Can I Find More?

 Schaaf, R., & Mailloux, Z. (2015). Clinician ’ s

Guide to Implementing Ayres Sensory Integration ® : Promoting Participation for Children

with Autism.

 A step-by-step guidebook that describes the

most effective, evidence-based way to implement ASI into clinical practice by using a

data-driven decision-making approach.

 DeMonia, L., & Turchan, M. (2015). Love for

Logan. San Antonio, TX : Halo Publishing

International.

 An inspirational story based on actual events.

A young girl learns to better understand why

day-to-day life can be challenging for her

older sister. A kid-friendly picture book, told

through the eyes of a sibling, will help children understand others’ sensory diffi culties,

and explain sensory processing disorder.

 Bialer, D., & Miller, L. J. (2011). No Longer A

SECRET : Unique Common Sense Strategies

for Children with Sensory or Motor Challenges. Arlington, TX: Sensory World.

 A resource for parents, teachers, and therapists

helping children with sensory or motor issues.

It includes cost-effective, functional, on the

spot tips to use for children with sensory

issues at home, at school, or in a community

setting.

 References

 Ayres , A. J. ( 1989 ). Sensory Integration and

Praxis Tests manual . Los Angeles, CA : Western

Psychological Services.

 Ayres , A. J. ( 2004 ). Sensory integration and the

child ( 2nd ed .). Los Angeles, CA : Western

Psychological Services.

 Bagby , M. , Dickie , V. , & Baranek , G. T. ( 2012 ). How

sensory experiences in children with and without

autism affect family occupations . American

Journal of Occupational Therapy , 66 , 78 – 86 .

 doi:10.5014/ajot.2012.000604

 Baranek , G. T. , Chin , Y. H. , Hess , L. M. G. , Yankee ,

 J. G. , Harton , D. D. , & Hooper , S. R. ( 2002 ).

 Sensory processing correlates of occupational

performance in children with Fragile X syndrome:

Preliminary fi ndings . American Journal of

Occupational Therapy , 56 , 538 – 546 . doi:10.5014/

ajot.56.5.538

 Baranek , G. T. , David , F. J. , Poe , M. D. , Stone , W. L. ,

& Watson , L. R. ( 2006 ). Sensory Experiences

Questionnaire: Discriminating sensory features

in young children with autism, developmental

delays, and typical development. Journal of Child

Psychology and Psychiatry , 47 ( 6 ), 591 – 601 .

 doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01546.x

 Bellefeuille , I. B. , Schaaf , R. C. , & Polo , E. R.

( 2013 ). Occupational therapy based on Ayres

Sensory Integration in the treatment of retentive

fecal incontinence in a 3-year-old boy . American

Journal of Occupational Therapy , 67 ( 5 ), 601 – 606 .

 doi:10.5014/ajot.2013.008086

 Benson , J. , Nicka , M. , & Stern , P. ( 2006 ). How does

a child with sensory processing problems play?

Internet Journal of Allied Health Sciences and

Practice , 4 , 1 – 6 .

 Blissett , J. , & Fogel , A. ( 2013 ). Intrinsic and

extrinsic infl uences on children ’ s acceptance of

new foods . Physiology & Behavior , 121 , 89 – 95 .

 doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.02.013

 Brown , T. ( 2012 ). Assessment, measurement and

evaluation: Why can ’ t I do what everyone expects

me to do? In S. J. Lane & A. C. Bundy ( Eds .),

Kids can be kids: A childhood occupations

approach ( pp . 320 – 348 ). Philadelphia, PA : F.A.

Davis.

 Bulkeley , K. , Bundy , A. , Roberts , J. , & Einfeld , S.

( 2016 ). Family-centered management of sensory

challenges of children with autism: A singlecase experimental design . American Journal

of Occupational Therapy , 70 , 7005220040 .

 doi:10.5014/ajot.2016.017822

 Bundy , A. C. ( 1989 ). A comparison of the play skills

of normal boys and boys with sensory integrative

dysfunction. OTJR: Occupation, Participation,

and Health , 9 , 84 – 100 .

 Bundy , A. C. , Shia , S. , Qi , L. , & Miller , L. J. ( 2007 ).

 How does sensory processing dysfunction affect

play? American Journal of Occupational

Therapy , 61 , 201 – 208 . doi:10.50144/

ajot.61.2.201

 Christensen , J. S. ( 2012 ). Early adolescent nonsuicidal self-injury and sensory preference

differences: An exploratory study . CGU Theses

& Dissertations , Paper 66 . Retrieved from http://

scholarship.claremont.edu/cgu_etd/66

CHAPTER 2 Sensory Integration in Everyday Life ■ 37

 Clifford , J. M. , & Bundy , A. C. ( 1989 ). Play

preference and play performance in normal boys

and boys with sensory integrative dysfunction.

OTJR: Occupation, Participation, and Health , 9 ,

 202 – 217 .

 Cohn , E. S. , Kramer , J. A. , Schub , J. A. , &

May-Benson, T. ( 2014 ). Parents’ explanatory

models and hopes for outcomes of occupational

therapy using a sensory integration approach .

American Journal of Occupational Therapy , 68 ,

 454 – 462 . doi:10.5014/ajot.2014.010843

 Cosbey , J. , Johnston , S. S. , & Dunn , M. L. ( 2010 ).

 Sensory processing disorders and social

participation. American Journal of Occupational

Therapy , 64 ( 3 ), 462 – 473 . doi:10.5014/

ajot.2010.09076

 Cosbey , J. , Johnston , S. S. , Dunn , M. L. , & Bauman ,

 M. ( 2012 ). Playground behaviors of children with

and without sensory processing disorders. OTJR:

Occupation, Participation, and Health , 32 , 39 – 47 .

 doi:10.3928/15394492-20110930-01

 Coster , W. ( 1998 ). Occupation-centered assessment

of children. American Journal of Occupational

Therapy , 52 , 337 – 344 . doi:10.5014/

ajot.52.5.337

 Davis , A. M. , Bruce , A. S. , Khasawneh , R. , Schulz ,

 T. , Fox , C. , & Dunn , W. ( 2013 ). Sensory

processing issues in young children presenting to

an outpatient feeding clinic: A retrospective chart

review . Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology

and Nutrition , 56 ( 2 ), 156 . doi:10.1097/

mpg.0b013e3182736e19

 DeSantis , A. , Coster , W. , Bigsby , R. , & Lester , B.

( 2004 ). Colic and fussing in infancy, and sensory

processing at 3 to 8 years of age . Infant Mental

Health Journal , 25 ( 6 ), 522 – 539 . doi:10.1002/

imhj.20025

 Dunbar , S. B. ( 1999 ). A child ’ s occupational

performance: Considerations of sensory processing

and family context. American Journal of

Occupational Therapy , 53 , 231 – 235 . doi:10.5014/

ajot.53.2.231

 Dunn , W. ( 1997 ). The impact of sensory

processing abilities on the daily lives of

young children and families: A conceptual

model . Infants and Young Children , 9 , 23 – 25 .

 doi:10.1097/00001163-199704000-00005

 Dunn , W. ( 2014 ). Sensory Profi le 2: User ’ s manual .

 San Antonio, TX : Psychological Corporation .

 Dunn , W. , Cox , J. , Foster , L. , Mische-Lawson , L. ,

& Tanquary , J. ( 2012 ). Impact of a contextual

intervention on child participation and parent

competence among children with autism spectrum

disorders: A pretest–posttest repeated-measures

design. American Journal of Occupational

Therapy , 66 ( 5 ), 520 – 528 . doi:10.5014/

ajot.2012.004119

 Engel-Yeger , B. , & Dunn , W. ( 2011a ). Exploring the

relationship between affect and sensory processing

patterns in adults . British Journal of Occupational

Therapy , 74 ( 10 ), 456 – 464 . doi:10.4276/

030802211x13182481841868

 Engel-Yeger , B. , & Dunn , W. ( 2011b ). The

relationship between sensory processing

diffi culties and anxiety level of healthy adults .

British Journal of Occupational Therapy ,

74 ( 5 ), 210 – 216 . doi:10.4276/0308022

11x13046730116407

 Engel-Yeger , B. , & Shochat , T. ( 2012 ). The

relationship between sensory processing patterns

and sleep quality in healthy adults . Canadian

Journal of Occupational Therapy , 79 ( 3 ), 134 – 141 .

 doi:10.2182/cjot.2012.79.3.2

 Esdaile , S. A. , & Olson , J. A. ( 2003 ). Mothering

occupations . Philadelphia, PA : F. A. Davis .

 Fanchiang , S.-P. ( 1996 ). The other side of the coin:

Growing up with a learning disability . American

Journal of Occupational Therapy , 50 , 277 – 285 .

 doi:10.5014/ajot.50.4.277

 Fanchiang , S.-P. , Snyder , C. , Zobel-Lachiusa , J. ,

 Loeffl er , C. B. , & Thompson , M. E. ( 1990 ).

 Sensory integrative processing in delinquent-prone

and non-delinquent-prone adolescents. American

Journal of Occupational Therapy , 44 ( 7 ), 630 – 639 .

 doi:10.5014/ajot.44.7.630

 Fisher , A. G. ( 1998 ). Uniting practice and theory in

an occupational framework. The 1998 Eleanor

Clark Slagle lecture. American Journal of

Occupational Therapy , 52 , 509 – 521 . doi:10.5014/

ajot.52.7.509

 Fisher , A. G. , & Marterella , A. ( 2019 ). Powerful

practice: A model of authentic occupational

therapy . Fort Collins, CO : Innovative

Occupational Therapy Practice.

 Fisher , A. G. , & Short-DeGraff , M. ( 1993 ).

 Improving functional assessment in occupational

therapy: Recommendations and philosophy

for change. American Journal of Occupational

Therapy , 47 , 199 – 200 . doi:10.5014/ajot.47.3.199

 Geva , R. , & Feldman , R. ( 2008 ). A neurobiological

model for the effects of early brainstem

functioning on the development of behavior

and emotion regulation in infants: Implications

for prenatal and perinatal risk . Journal of Child

Psychology and Psychiatry , 49 ( 10 ), 1031 – 1041 .

 doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01918.x

 Grandin , T. ( 2006 ). Thinking in pictures: My life

with autism ( 2nd ed. ). New York, NY :

Vintage Books .

 Gray , J. M. ( 1998 ). Putting occupation into practice:

Occupation as ends, occupation as means.

American Journal of Occupational Therapy , 52 ,

 354 – 364 . doi:10.5014/ajot.52.5.354

 Green , S. A. , & Ben-Sasson , A. ( 2010 ). Anxiety

disorders and sensory over-responsivity in

children with autism spectrum disorders: Is

there a causal relationship? Journal of Autism

and Developmental Disorders , 40 , 1495 – 1504 .

 doi:10.1007/s10803-010-1007-x

 Greene , K. , Krcmar , M. , Walters , L. H. , Rubin , D. L. ,

& Hale , L. ( 2000 ). Targeting adolescent risktaking behaviors: The contributions of egocentrism

and sensation-seeking. Journal of Adolescence ,

23 ( 4 ), 439 – 461 . doi:10.1006/jado.2000.0330

38 ■ PART I Theoretical Constructs

 Hartshorn , K. , Olds , L. , Field , T. , Delage , J. ,

 Cullen , C. , & Escalona , A. ( 2001 ). Creative

movement therapy benefi ts children with autism .

Early Child Development and Care , 166 , 1 – 5 .

 doi:10.1080/0300443011660101

 Hilton , C. , Graver , K. , & LaVesser , P. ( 2007 ).

 Relationship between social competence and

sensory processing in children with high

functioning autism spectrum disorders. Research

in Autism Spectrum Disorders , 1 ( 2 ), 164 – 173 .

 doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2006.10.002

 Hochhauser , M. , & Engel-Yeger , B. ( 2010 ).

 Sensory processing abilities and their relation

to participation in leisure activities among

children with high-functioning autism spectrum

disorder (HFASD). Research in Autism Spectrum

Disorders , 4 ( 4 ), 746 – 754 . doi:10.1016/j.

rasd.2010.01.015

 Hofer , M. A. ( 2006 ). Psychobiological roots

of early attachment. Current Directions

in Psychological Science , 15 ( 2 ), 84 – 88 .

 doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00412.x

 Jerome , E. M. , & Liss , M. ( 2005 ). Relationships

between sensory processing style, adult

attachment, and coping . Personality and

Individual Differences , 38 ( 6 ), 1341 – 1352 .

 doi:10.1016/j.paid.2004.08.016

 Kientz , M. , & Dunn , W. ( 2012 ). Evaluating the

effectiveness of contextual intervention for

adolescents with autism spectrum disorders.

Journal of Occupational Therapy, Schools, &

Early Intervention , 5 ( 3–4 ), 196 – 208 . doi:10.1080/

19411243.2012.737271

 Kimball , J. G. , Birstler , C. T. , Bosse , E. F. , Nelson ,

 L. M. , & Woods , M. R. ( 2012 ). The relationships

among sensory processing styles, personality

traits, and body mass index: A pilot study .

Occupational Therapy in Mental Health , 28 ( 1 ),

 72 – 87 . doi:10.1080/0164212x.2012.650994

 Kinnealey , M. , Koenig , K. P. , & Smith , S. ( 2011 ).

 Relationships between sensory modulation and

social supports and health-related quality of life .

American Journal of Occupational Therapy ,

65 ( 3 ), 320 – 327 . doi:10.5014/ajot.2011.001370

 Kinnealey , M. , Oliver , B. , & Wilbarger , P. ( 1995 ). A

phenomenological study of sensory defensiveness

in adults . American Journal of Occupational

Therapy , 49 , 444 – 451 . doi:10.5014/ajot.49.5.444

 Koenig , K. P. , Buckley-Reen , A. , & Garg , S. ( 2012 ).

 Effi cacy of the Get Ready to Learn Yoga Program

among children with autism spectrum disorders:

A pretest-posttest control group design . American

Journal of Occupational Therapy , 66 , 538 – 546 .

 doi:10.5014/ajot.2012.004390

 Koenig , K. P. , & Rudney , S. G. ( 2010 ). Performance

challenges for children and adolescents with

diffi culty processing and integrating sensory

information: A systematic review . American

Journal of Occupational Therapy , 64 , 430 – 442 .

 doi:10.5014/ajot.2010.09073

 Lane , A. E. , Young , R. L. , Baker , A. E. , & Angley ,

 M. T. ( 2010 ). Sensory processing subtypes in

autism: Association with adaptive behavior .

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders ,

40 ( 1 ), 112 – 122 . doi:10.1007/s10803-009-0840-2

 Leekam , S. , Nieto , C. , Libby , S. , Wing , L. , & Gould ,

 J. ( 2007 ). Describing the sensory abnormalities of

children and adults with autism. Journal of Autism

and Developmental Disorders , 37 , 894 – 910 .

 doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0218-7

 Lightfoot , C. ( 1997 ). The culture of adolescent risktaking . New York, NY : The Guilford Press .

 Mattingly , C. , & Fleming , M. ( 1994 ). Clinical

reasoning: Forms of inquiry in a therapeutic

process . Philadelphia, PA : F. A. Davis .

 McCarter , J. A. ( 2010 ). Growing up with sensory

processing challenges. AOTA Special Interest

Section Quarterly: Sensory Integration , 33 ( 3 ),

 1 – 2 .

 Miller , L. J. , Coll , J. R. , & Schoen , S. A. ( 2007 ).

 A randomized controlled pilot study of the

effectiveness of occupational therapy for children

with sensory modulation disorder . American

Journal of Occupational Therapy , 61 , 228 – 238 .

 doi:10.5014/ajot.61.2.228

 Miller Kuhaneck , H. , & Britner , P. A. ( 2013 ). A

preliminary investigation of the relationship

between sensory processing and social play in

autism spectrum disorder . OTJR: Occupation,

Participation and Health , 33 ( 3 ), 159 – 167 .

 Mische Lawson , L. , & Dunn , W. ( 2008 ). Children ’ s

sensory processing patterns and play preferences.

Annual in Therapeutic Recreation , 16 , 1 – 14 .

 O’Neil , M. ( 2010 ). Parenting a child with sensory

integration challenges. SISQ: Sensory Integration ,

33 ( 3 ), 2 – 3 .

 Parham , L. D. ( 1998 ). The relationship of sensory

integrative development to achievement in

elementary students: Four-year longitudinal

patterns. Occupational Therapy Journal of

Research , 18 , 105 – 127 .

 Pfeiffer , B. A. , Koenig , K. , Kinnealey , M. , Sheppard ,

 M. , & Henderson , L. ( 2011 ). Effectiveness of

sensory integration interventions in children

with autism spectrum disorders: A pilot study .

American Journal of Occupational Therapy , 65 ,

 76 – 85 . doi:10.5014/ajot.2011.09205

 Pierce , D. ( 2009 ). Co-occupations: The challenges

of defi ning concepts original to occupational

science. Journal of Occupational Science , 16 ,

 203 – 207 .

 Pohl , P. S. , Dunn , W. , & Brown , C. ( 2003 ). The role

of sensory processing in the everyday lives of

older adults . OTJR: Occupation, Participation,

and Health , 23 , 99 – 106 .

 Purvis , K. B. , McKenzie , L. B. , Cross , D. R. , &

 Razuri , E. B. ( 2013 ). A spontaneous emergence

of attachment behavior in at - risk children and

a correlation with sensory defi cits . Journal of

Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Nursing , 26 ( 3 ),

 165 – 172 . doi:10.1111/jcap.12041

 Radzyminski , S. ( 2005 ). Neurobehavioral

functioning and breastfeeding behavior in the

newborn. Journal of Obstetric, Gynecologic,

CHAPTER 2 Sensory Integration in Everyday Life ■ 39

& Neonatal Nursing , 34 ( 3 ), 335 – 341 .

 doi:10.1177/0884217505276283

 Reynolds , S. , Bendixen , R. M. , Lawrence , T. , &

 Lane , S. ( 2011 ). A pilot study examining activity

participation, sensory responsiveness, and

competence in children with high functioning

autism spectrum disorder . Journal of Autism

and Developmental Disorders , 41 , 1496 – 1506 .

 doi:10.1007/s10803-010-1173-x

 Reynolds , S. , & Lane , S. J. ( 2008 ). Diagnostic

validity of sensory over-responsivity: A review of

the literature and case reports. Journal of Autism

and Developmental Disorders , 38 , 516 – 529 .

 doi:10.1007/s10803-007-0418-9

 Reynolds , S. , & Lane , S. J. ( 2009 ). Sensory overresponsivity and anxiety in children with ADHD.

American Journal of Occupational Therapy , 63 ,

 443 – 450 . doi:10.5014/ajot.63.4.433

 Reynolds , S. , Lane , S. J. , & Thacker , L. ( 2011 ).

 Sensory processing, physiological stress, and

sleep behaviors in children with and without

autism spectrum disorders. OTJR: Occupational,

Participation, and Health , 32 , 246 – 257 .

 doi:10.3928/15394492-20110513-02

 Sameroff , A. ( 2009 ). The transactional model . In

 A. Sameroff ( Ed .), The transactional model of

development: How children and contexts shape

each other ( pp . 3 – 21 ). Washington, DC : American

Psychological Association.

 Schaaf , R. C. ( 2011 ). Interventions that address

sensory dysfunction for individuals with autism

spectrum disorders: Preliminary evidence for

the superiority of sensory integration compared

to other sensory approaches. In B. Reichow , P.

 Doehring , D. V. Cicchetti , & F. R. Volkmar ( Eds .),

Evidence-based practices and treatments for

children with autism ( pp . 245 – 273 ). New York,

NY : Springer .

 Schaaf , R. C. , Benevides , T. , Mailloux , Z. , Faller , P. ,

 Hunt , J. , van Hooydonk , E. , … Kelly , D. ( 2014 ).

 An intervention for sensory diffi culties in children

with autism: A randomized trial . Journal of Autism

and Developmental Disabilities , 44 , 1493 – 1506 .

 doi:10.1007/s10803-013-1983-8

 Shea , C. , & Wu , R. ( 2012 ). Examining the sensory

profi les of at-risk youth participating in a preemployment program. The Open Journal of

Occupational Therapy , 1 ( 1 ), 5 .

 Shochat , T. , Tzischinsky , O. , & Engel-Yeger ,

 B. ( 2009 ). Sensory hypersensitivity as a

contributing factor in the relation between

sleep and behavioral disorders in normal

children. Behavioral Sleep Medicine , 7 , 53 – 62 .

 doi:10.1080/15402000802577777

 Smyth , M. M. , & Anderson , H. I. ( 2000 ). Coping

with clumsiness in the school playground:

Social and physical play in children with

coordination impairments. British Journal of

Developmental Psychology , 18 ( 3 ), 389 – 413 .

 doi:10.1348/026151000165760

 Stols , D. , Van Heerden , R. , Van Jaarsveld , A. , & Nel ,

 R. ( 2013 ). Substance abusers’ anger behaviour

and sensory processing patterns: An

occupational therapy investigation. South

African Journal of Occupational Therapy , 43 ( 1 ),

 25 – 34 .

 Thomas , S. , Bundy , A. C. , Black , D. , & Lane ,

 S. J. ( 2015 ). Toward early identifi cation of

sensory over-responsivity (SOR): A construct

for predicting diffi culties with sleep and

feeding in infants. OTJR: Occupation,

Participation and Health , 35 , 178 – 186 .

 doi:10.1177/1539449215579855

 Trombly , C. A. ( 1993 ). Anticipating the future:

Assessment of occupational function.

American Journal of Occupational Therapy ,

47 , 253 – 257 .

 Trott , M. C. , Laurel , M. K. , & Windeck , S. L.

( 1993 ). SenseAbilities: Understanding sensory

integration . Tucson, AZ : Therapy Skill Builders

( now ProEd ).

 Turner , K. A. , Cohn , E. S. , & Koomar , J. ( 2012 ).

 Mothering when mothers and children both have

sensory processing challenges. British Journal of

Occupational Therapy , 75 ( 10 ), 449 – 455 . doi:10.4

276/030802212X13496921049626

 Watts , T. , Stagnitti , K. , & Brown , T. ( 2014 ).

 Relationship between play and sensory processing:

A systematic review . American Journal of

Occupational Therapy , 68 , e37 – e46 . doi:10.5014/

ajot.2014.009787

 Weiss-Salinas , D. , & Williams , N. ( 2001 ). Sensory

defensiveness: A theory of its effect on

breastfeeding. Journal of Human Lactation , 17 ( 2 ),

 145 – 151 . doi:10.1177/089033440101700211

 Welters-Davis , M. , & Mische Lawson , L. ( 2011 ).

 The relationship between sensory processing

and parent-child play preferences. Journal of

Occupational Therapy, Schools, and Early

Intervention , 4 , 108 – 120 . doi:10.1080/19411243.2

011.595300

 Wiener , A. S. , Long , T. , DeGangi , G. A. , & Battaile ,

 B. ( 1996 ). Sensory processing of infants born

prematurely or with regulatory disorders. Physical

& Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics , 16 ( 4 ),

 1 – 18 .

 Williams , M. S. , & Shellenberger , S. ( 1996 ). How

does your engine run? A leader ’ s guide to the

Alert Program for Self-regulation . Albuquerque,

NM : TherapyWorks .

 Zemke , R. , & Clark , F. ( 1996 ). Co-occupations of

mothers and children. In R. Zemke & F. Clark

( Eds .), Occupational science: The evolving

discipline ( pp . 213 – 215 ). Philadelphia, PA : F.A.

Davis.

 Zuckerman , M. ( 1994 ). Behavioral expressions and

biosocial bases of sensation seeking . Cambridge,

UK : Cambridge University Press .

40

CHAPTER

3

Composing a Theory

 An Historical Perspective

 Shelly J. Lane , PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA ■ Anita C. Bundy , ScD, OT/L, FAOTA ■ Michael E. Gorman , PhD

 Chapter 3

Upon completion of this chapter, the reader will be able to:

✔ Recognize how Ayres’ personal traits and

professional training led to the development of

sensory integration (SI) theory.

✔ Describe how a community of clinicians and

scholars, led by Ayres, emerged and grew the

fi eld of SI within occupational therapy.

✔ Identify how SI research and practice has

evolved while continuing to be grounded in

Ayres’ original theory.

 Take criticism seriously, but not personally. If there is truth or merit in

the criticism, try to learn from it. Otherwise, let it roll right off you.

 — Hillary Clinton

LEARNING OUTCOMES

Purpose and Scope

 Ayres’ work has been, and continues to be,

admired by some and rejected by others.

Throughout her professional life she seemed to

take both in stride. Although the rejections and

misunderstandings were distressing to her, she

had learned in childhood how to withdraw into

her own mind and to use willpower to get past

unhappiness. She worked, studied, and read

relentlessly, never certain how much time she

would have to complete her life goals ( Gorman

& Kashani, 2017 ; Sieg, 1988 ).

 Endeavoring to understand both the history

of sensory integration (SI) and current research

and practice trends, we interviewed colleagues

who had the opportunity to learn, study, and

work with Ayres and were infl uential in shaping

SI theory and related concepts. We have also

conducted in-depth reviews of related literature.

Based on this work, we conclude that, although

we have seen evolution, the core construct of

Ayres’ Sensory Integration ® (ASI) theory, linking

brain processing with observable behavior, continues to be a focal point in theory and practice.

 Here we present a historical account of the

evolution of SI theory and practice, drawn from

our interviews and readings. We have chosen

to acknowledge contributors at the close of the

chapter rather than attribute individual comments

to individual colleagues.

A Little Background

 ASI theory is globally used in pediatric practice,

and core constructs are applied across many other

areas of practice. This theoretical foundation has

engendered much research and collegial discourse, likely more than any other theory in the

profession of occupational therapy ( May-Benson

& Koomar, 2010 ; Schaaf et al., 2015 ). SI theory

emerged because of Ayres’ constant imperative to

look beyond behavior and gain an understanding

CHAPTER 3 Composing a Theory ■ 41

of the neurological underpinnings of disorders, in

order to support engagement and participation in

the activities of daily life. Blending research with

clinical practice, the children with whom Ayres

interacted were her motivation to link neuroscience and behavior. An emergent leader around

whom people congregated, Ayres embodied the

roles of clinician, researcher, and teacher, always

encouraging others to do the same.

 According to Kielhofner ( 2004 ), Ayres’ work

evolved at a time of “crisis” in occupational

therapy. Up to this point, roughly through the

1960s, occupational therapy had focused on the

importance of occupation in daily life, using

“doing” as therapy. However, the medical fi eld

indicated that, although it seemed apparent that

engagement in occupation was benefi cial, there

was no comprehensive theory and no documented evidence that occupational therapy was

effective ( Kielhofner, 2004 ). Ayres, in the mid- to

late 1950s, had begun her work examining proprioceptive facilitation for the upper extremities,

with an interest in understanding the underlying

mechanisms of both function and dysfunction.

Kielhofner suggested that Ayres became part of

a newly emerging mechanistic paradigm in occupational therapy, emphasizing that the benefi ts

of engagement in occupation and activity were

best explained by understanding the underlying

mechanisms. Although Ayres was highly invested

in understanding the neurological mechanisms

of sensory integrative function and dysfunction,

she never lost sight of the bigger picture, that of

supporting engagement in childhood occupations

(see Fig. 3-1 ).

 Ayres may not have set out to become the

“heart” of SI. In fact, accounts suggest that

she wanted anything but to become the center

of this universe; rather, she preferred that there

be centers of sensory integrative knowledge all

around the world. Nonetheless, she was driven

to understand the underpinnings of the disorders

she saw clinically. She pursued advanced training in research and neuroscience to guide her

growing knowledge of links between neurophysiological mechanisms and clinical practice, and,

through time, developed the theory and practice

of SI. She became the hub of knowledge about

SI. According to Kielhofner ( 2004 ), the model

of SI became a “conceptual practice model.”

Such models emerge as a means of explaining

phenomena, developing tools for practice, and

setting the stage for research on the model. By

their very nature, these models are dynamic; they

are revised in an organic way as new knowledge

develops through both research and clinical

application. This is certainly true of SI.

Ayres the Person

 Sieg ( 1988 ) summarized Ayres’ childhood as one

in which she found comfort in being outdoors

on the family farm and enjoyed being with her

brother and younger sister. Ayres reportedly had

a diffi cult relationship with an older sister as well

as her mother, becoming increasingly introverted

in response to these relationships. She also grew

up dealing with what she saw as a “damaged

left hemisphere,” which caused her to struggle

to understand spoken words, particularly if the

speaker had an accent ( Sieg , 1988 ). Although

Ayres went on to do a great deal of reading,

writing, and speaking, writing and lecturing

remained a challenge for her throughout her life

FIGURE 3-1 Ayres, always a clinician, “learning”

from a client. Used with permission by Franklin B.

Baker/A. Jean Ayres Baker Trust.

42 ■ PART I Theoretical Constructs

( Fig. 3-2 ). According to one colleague, Ayres

“didn ’ t like lecturing, she didn ’ t like writing,

but she had to do it because she wanted to do

her research . . . if you ever look at one of her

lectures, they ’ re color coded all the way through

. . . she needed that coding visually to get her

through her lectures.”

 Only a small number of therapists that

we interviewed felt they truly knew Ayres

as a person. She was described as “. . . [not]

social-social, but she was very relational.” Ayres

was noted to be amazingly kind and an excellent

listener. In spite of what may have been a childhood challenged by many factors (including her

own health), Ayres put family, and her husband,

Franklin, at the center of her world: “Her family

was probably central . . . her family was like her

nest and her comfort. That was like a cocoon for

her . . . she never really talked about her family

or anything. [But] in her writing you can see

that was her main core heart of her life. . . .”

Her love of family is refl ected in Love, Jean,

 published after her death. This compendium of

letters between herself and her nephew, Phillip,

refl ects her love and concern for him as they

worked together to treat his sensory integrative

disorder, “by remote control,” through letters.

Her dedication to Franklin became apparent in

Sieg ’ s narrative, in which Ayres stated, “I have a

nearly perfect, just nearly perfect marriage. Just

a real love relationship. A complete pair bond”

( Sieg, 1988 , p. 96). Ayres’ fi rst book, Sensory

Integration and Learning Disorders, was simply

dedicated, “To Franklin.”

Ayres the Professional: Developing

Her Knowledge Base

 Most colleagues that we interviewed offered

more insight into Ayres as a therapist and scientist

than Ayres on a personal level. Ayres completed

her coursework in occupational therapy in 1944

at the University of Southern California (USC),

and she performed her clinical internships in

1946, the same year that she successfully passed

the occupational therapy registration examination. Working clinically in psychiatry (Birmingham Veterans Hospital, Brentwood Sanatorium)

and rehabilitation (Kabat-Kaiser Institute) fueled

her already established need for knowledge.

Her investment in occupational therapy drove

her to investigate underlying components of

the disabilities that she saw in her patients; she

wanted to better understand the cause so she

could better focus the treatment. She began with

adults, in rehabilitation, and as such was always

invested in a better understanding of neurological functions. In 1949, Ayres fi rst published a

paper on craft analysis following electroshock

treatments ( Ayres, 1949 ). She became interested

in hand functions, publishing on this topic, and

her research career bloomed with her master ’ s

degree awarded by USC. Her thesis comprised a

series of three manuscripts addressing her work

on proprioceptive facilitation of the extremities

in occupational therapy, published in the American Journal of Occupational Therapy in 1955

( Ayres, 1955a, 1955b, 1955c ). Ayres had been

inspired by her mentor, Margaret Rood. Rood,

with degrees in both occupational and physical

therapy, was a motor control theorist, one of the

fi rst. Rood ’ s focus on refl exes, and her emphasis

on the use of proprioceptive inhibition and facilitation, led Ayres to begin her research on the

basics of proprioception from a neurological perspective, as well as the application of this basic

science information to occupational therapy. In

the conclusion of her fi nal paper, Ayres stated:

 It seems, then, that the fundamental organization of the neuromuscular system is based on

function. When something disturbs that fundamental organization, it is reasonable to presume

that treatment might well be based on function—on activities simulating simple, normal,

life-like processes utilizing neurophysiological

mechanisms recognized for the integrative role

they play. . . . [While] this sounds very logical,

it has not been found to be entirely practical.

FIGURE 3-2 Although Ayres did not enjoy writing,

she knew it was crucial to move her work forward.

Used with permission by Franklin B. Baker/A. Jean

Ayres Baker Trust.

CHAPTER 3 Composing a Theory ■ 43

This suggests that the treatment of neuromuscular disorders is in the early developmental

stages. . . . Treatment procedures will have to

adjust accordingly as progress is made. ( Ayres,

 1955c , pp. 125–126)

 Such a statement captures core characteristics

that underlie Ayres’ subsequent work: a grounding in neuroscience, a focus on the importance of

sensation and SI in the production of movement,

an appreciation for the importance of engagement in daily life activities including play, and

the recognition that there is much to understand

related to implementing occupational therapy.

 Ayres gained further clinical experience in

rehabilitation, and she worked at the United

Cerebral Palsy Pre-nursery School and Vocational Training Center before returning to USC

to pursue her doctoral degree. In 1955, she

joined the Occupational Therapy faculty at USC,

continuing there until 1964. While pursuing

her PhD in Educational Psychology, completed

in 1961, Ayres’ interest in the neuroscience of

sensory systems moved her toward examining

visual spatial abilities and body scheme. During

this time, she developed an appreciation of the

need for tools that would enable her, and other

clinicians, to better understand the role played

by perceptual and motor processes in learning.

Ayres had developed the Motor Accuracy test

before entering her doctoral program, and she

went on to focus on other tests of visual perception, developing the Ayres Space Test through

her doctoral degree ( Gorman & Kashani, 2017 ).

Linking her clinical work with the neuroscience

literature led her to a deeper understanding of the

integrative nature of sensory processing; Ayres

determined that visual perception relied heavily

on the processing of vestibular and proprioceptive

inputs ( Sieg, 1988 ). “The employing of neural

mechanisms to enhance motor development is

now well established; the current area of major

growth and controversy lies in the use of neurological constructs to aid in understanding and

ameliorating cognitive functions such as learning

disabilities; the next step may well be a more

fruitful attack on emotional and behavior disorders” ( Henderson, Llorens, Gilfoyle, Myers, &

Prevel, 1974 , p. xii). Ayres completed a postdoctoral fellowship at the Brain Research Institute

(BRI) at UCLA from 1964 through 1966, where

she could study with neuroscientists and expand

her thinking on brain function and dysfunction.

Although the neuroscientists did not uniformly

accept her ideas and perspective, the BRI provided a great learning opportunity and environment for her ( Sieg, 1988 ). During this same time

frame, she was working on, and deeply passionate about, her fi rst text, Sensory Integration and

Learning Disorders ( Ayres, 1972 ). One of Ayres’

colleagues indicated the following:

 The quandary of deciding what research to

include and what she needed to leave out was

something Jean talked about all the time. Whenever Jean talked about this future publication

the creative passion that fueled her capacity

for work visibly energized her body; her eyes

brightened, her face lit up, and everything about

her animated. It was amazing and beautiful

to watch.

 On the heels of completing her postdoctoral

fellowship, Ayres returned to USC. However,

Ayres reported not being accepted in occupational therapy, indicating “I became so disgusted

with occupational therapy in general because

I kept wanting to push the fi eld and the fi eld

pushed back. . . . I just couldn ’ t tolerate the negativism toward me” ( Sieg , 1988 , p. 97). Consequently, Ayres initially joined the Department of

Special Education; she was later appointed as

Adjunct in Occupational Therapy. She opened

the Ayres Clinic in Torrance, California, in 1977

and taught the fi rst SI treatment course linked

with USC, OT610 (see Fig. 3-3 ).

 By all accounts, Ayres was brilliant, insightful,

and committed, and she was considered a mentor

by many (see Fig 3-4 ), as is clearly evident in the

following statements from interviewees:

 She was such a wonderful mentor because not

only did she have the intelligence to formulize

her hypothesis, and it was way out there then,

but she designed the equipment; she designed

the tests. It was amazing. Plus, she developed

her clinical practice to do all the research.

 I am still learning from her and if I go back

to what she has written it astounds me sometimes. There will be one or two little sentences

and I will go, “Oh my God, I missed that.” . . .

Ayres has informed me about everything really.

 There ’ s so much that I learned from her

about just how you do science that I mean, it

was always with me.

 Ayres required that I ask her questions

during my mentorship with her (1973). She

said, “You must question me or you won ’ t be

able to question yourself. If you can ’ t question

44 ■ PART I Theoretical Constructs

FIGURE 3-3 Ayres at her clinic. Used with permission

by Franklin B. Baker/A. Jean Ayres Baker Trust.

FIGURE 3-4 Ayres with Virginia Scardina. Ginny

always viewed Ayres as a mentor who guided her

clinical career. Used with permission by Franklin B.

Baker/A. Jean Ayres Baker Trust.

yourself, you ’ ll never be a researcher.” She

gave me some really poignant and good advice.

 In our interviews, Ayres was described as

focused, perhaps driven by her need to understand herself. Ayres was strongly intrinsically

motivated in her work, and she appeared never

to rest her mind. “One thing Jean always did

was read the neurophysiological research. I mean

that was primary in her mind when she was

looking and developing and thinking in terms of

treatment and mechanisms and giving her lectures on theory and all. She was always reading

the literature.” Although it appeared that Ayres

spent a considerable amount of time reading and

applying the neurophysiological literature, her

interests and knowledge extended well beyond

neuroscience. “I learned at that time really how

widespread her reading was; I had no idea, I

thought she just lived neuroanatomy, not so. So, I

found that she was really [widely] read and very

experimental and experimented with herself over

everything.”

 Ayres tried many of the activities on herself

that she would use eventually with children; “She

had problems herself, and so she kind of was analyzing what she felt were underlying things in her

and then developed tests to test them. She was

that way all the way along.” We were told that it

was not easy for Ayres to try out her ideas: “One

time I said, ‘Jean, why are you doing this? How

come? It ’ s so hard!’ She said, ‘Do you think I

could stop if I wanted to?’ So, it was her inner

drive and I learned that about her.” Ayres was

seen to be in constant search of an answer to her

current burning questions and in search of a tool

or an approach that would help her focus her own

mind. “She was always trying to keep her own

brain focused and get the energy out. Then she

would come back later, ‘Damn, it wore out.’ She

habituated and it didn ’ t work out as well.”

 Importantly, Ayres was child-centered in all

her interactions with her young clients. In fact,

several interviewees commented on her way with

children, her deep ability to read their cues and

provide the support and challenge they needed to

do the things they wanted to do. These perspectives are refl ected in the following comments:

 I mean to watch her treat . . . that was something that just really kept striking me because

I knew she was this marvelous researcher and

test developer and everything, but I think to

watch her treat was just a gift because she could

CHAPTER 3 Composing a Theory ■ 45

just pick up exactly what the kid needed at that

point in time and do it.

 It was how she treated kids and how she

interacted with kids, and how she seemed to go

deeper with them . . . just watching her treat;

her quietness, and the trust that the children

had in her. Her presence with the children was

pretty amazing, as well as her way of bringing

the energy in the room to a just-right level.

 In addition to her sensitivity in working with

the children, she also had a way with parents.

This becomes evident in reading Love, Jean

 ( Ayres, 2004 ) as well as in statements such as the

following: “The greatest contribution that I think

Ayres has made is helping parents to understand

their children ’ s behaviors.”

 Throughout her career, Ayres mentored hundreds of practitioners, researchers, and theorists,

and she infl uenced countless more. Essentially,

against her will, Ayres became a transformational

leader ( Northouse, 2015 ), leading by example,

inspiring and motivating others to create change

in the fi eld of occupational therapy. She enjoyed

interaction with therapists who asked questions,

therapists who she could see thinking. We were

told, “She liked people asking, questioning, and

not believing her . . . not swallowing hook, line,

sinker.” Before her untimely death in 1988,

Ayres had presented widely on SI, and she had

published several manuscripts linking clinical

and research fi ndings, as well as several book

chapters, two distinctive texts, and two full

assessment batteries.

HERE ’ S THE POINT

• ASI theory is based on linking observable

behavior with neural underpinnings to support

engagement and participation.

• Ayres did not set out to be a famous scholar

but was driven by her desire to understand

herself and her work.

• Ayres’ teaching and mentorship of clinicians

and scholars has helped her theory evolve and

has created widespread change in the fi eld of

pediatric occupational therapy.

Growth of Sensory Integration

Theory and Research

 Until 1971, Ayres had been able to get private and

federal funding for her work. Even in the 1970s,

obtaining grant funding was no small feat, so this

accomplishment needs to be acknowledged. It is

quite likely that Ayres’ ability to obtain funding

was related to her research focus grounded in

hard science. However, once this support ceased,

Ayres knew she still needed to do research and

that it would require funding.

Research and the Center

for the Study of Sensory

Integrative Dysfunction

 In conjunction with colleagues, Dottie Ecker

and Sue Knox, Ayres began to generate research

dollars from teaching endeavors; by and large

in these early days, income from courses was

managed on an informal basis, with courses

run by a small committee. The initial courses

focused on perceptual motor skills rather than

SI. Ayres was the primary lecturer, although she

was joined by such fi gures as Margaret Rood

and, gradually, by other presenters, such as Knox

and Ecker. Aside from academic contributions,

Knox reported that she and Ecker also brought

“goodies” for course participants. As the teaching grew, the core group decided to form a nonprofi t organization into which funds from courses

could be placed and subsequently used to support

research. In 1972, with private funding, Ayres

formally established the Center for the Study

of Sensory Integrative Dysfunction (CSSID).

Lawrene Kovalenko spearheaded the establishment of CSSID, with the crucial support of Sue

Knox and Dottie Ecker. According to Knox, Pat

Wilbarger and Ayres were among the original

trustees, and the Education Committee consisted

of Dottie Ecker, Mary Silberzahn, and Maryann

Rinsch. Rinsch resigned soon after the establishment of CSSID, leaving an opening that Knox

fi lled. In addition to the large private donation

that Ayres secured for research, there were other

contributions. Sharon Cermak indicated she wrote

the fi rst check to CSSID, for $25, and in 1975

Judy Kimball sponsored Ayres to do a conference in Syracuse, New York, yielding more than

$8,500 in proceeds for CSSID, much to Ayres’

appreciation and delight. Although establishment

of a center did not sit well with Ayres, she felt it

was necessary to support ongoing research.

 Although CSSID started small, the demand

for courses grew such that additional teaching faculty were recruited beginning in 1974.

46 ■ PART I Theoretical Constructs

Simple courses morphed into conferences

with a certifi cate of attendance; these would

morph again into workshops leading to certifi cation. The certifi cation process required

additional faculty and, from this need, faculty

training developed. The fi rst training was held

in 1976, and gradually the faculty ranks grew.

During training, faculty listened to presentations

related to theory and testing, and Ayres shared

case studies and aspects of her current work

( Fig. 3-5 ). Many therapists currently associated

with SI, as well as therapists currently more

closely associated with sensory-based and complementary interventions, had their start with

CSSID. All contributed to the dissemination of

SI theory and practice and, to some extent, to its

research base. Faculty met yearly, sharing developing knowledge. One colleague refl ected, “We

were literally a think tank, and not only did we

talk about teaching the course, we also talked

about theory and treatment. We talked about

the research.”

 In these early years, there were great learning opportunities and a great deal of sharing

among faculty. The need for courses continued,

and the number of CSSID faculty grew. Therapists around the country applied, or were invited,

to join this perceived “elite” group of instructors. Not everyone who wanted to be part of

the faculty was included, and some individuals

maintain they were actively discouraged or even

blackballed from becoming faculty. At some

point there was a moratorium on adding additional faculty, creating a sense of those who were

“in” and those who were not, fostering unrest

and potentially adding fuel to a smoldering fi re.

 In 1983, CSSID became Sensory Integration

International (SII). This was reportedly prompted

because there was increasingly “. . . big international interest . . . some of the faculty being

international [e.g., Canadians] . . . and they

wanted more of . . . a name that didn ’ t sound so

small and isolated. . . .” The name change was

not a point of contention; faculty supported the

need to have a name that captured their growing

international presence.

SII and Growing Tension

 Courses offered by SII were doing well, and the

Ayres Clinic appeared to be thriving. However,

Ayres, the reluctant but admired leader, was battling cancer for the second time in her short life.

When she received the second diagnosis, she

began to withdraw from activities not directly

related to completing the Sensory Integration

and Praxis Test (SIPT; Ayres, 1989 ) . Perhaps in

response to the impending loss of Ayres and her

separation from the role of “leader,” colleagues

began to move in different directions. According

to Ballinger ( 2007 ), this is to be expected with

leader succession. The response of members

within the group varies depending on their relationship with the leader. Ballinger indicated that

responses of those closest to the leader likely

include loyalty, respect, and high levels of affect,

whereas individuals farther from the core fi nd

themselves less affectively infl uenced by the

impending loss. With regard to the fi eld of SI,

this appears to be a time when many sensoryintegration-informed, but more sensory-based,

therapies began to develop, and when schisms

among and between individuals and groups

became more apparent.

 With her illness, Ayres planned to sell the

Ayres Clinic, offering it fi rst to USC. Although

the Occupational Therapy Department seriously

considered the purchase, it did not happen, and

in 1985 SII purchased the Ayres Clinic. USC Professor Florence Clark became the director, maintaining the strong link with USC until 1989 when

she resigned to concentrate on the establishment

of the Occupational Science PhD program. This

was something that “Jean would have wanted,

that her clinic would be associated with the university and led by a prominent scholar with a

FIGURE 3-5 Ayres teaching OT610 in the Ayres

Clinic. Used with permission by Franklin B. Baker/A.

Jean Ayres Baker Trust.

CHAPTER 3 Composing a Theory ■ 47

PhD.” Others who had long been associated with

both OT610 and the Ayres Clinic (Parham and

Mailloux) maintained their roles.

 Unfortunately, SII administrative leadership

at this time was somewhat divisive and unstable.

Following removal of one director, there was a

period when things went well; “[Steve Leinau]

came in . . . and he put [SII] on the map. He

began doing international conferences, and the

fi rst one I went to was Marian Diamond and

Temple Grandin and not a lot of people knew

about them then. So, he started doing conferences, started publishing and doing all that, for

about 2½ years. . . .” Steve moved on, and a

series of executive directors passed through the

organization, one who provided stability and

forward momentum, and others who had considerably less positive infl uence, possibly engaging

in unethical practice. The SIPT was published in

1988; new certifi cation courses were introduced;

and practitioners were required to retool, another

source of tension. Several changes took place in

the SII Board of Directors through the ensuing

years, and power appeared to shift. There was a

sense that some members of the Board had an

agenda that was not consistent with the direction of the organization; “They kind of had

other interests as well, sensory integration and

not strict areas of SI kind of things. . . . They

started micromanaging the clinic . . . telling . . .

the group what they could do and they couldn ’ t

do and everything else.” Differences developed between clinicians in the Ayres Clinic and

members of the Board of Directors relative to

the clinical application of SI, with the board supporting what would be termed “sensory-based”

approaches and the clinic therapists feeling that

this deviated too far from the core principles of

SI. Following what was described as “a very

uncomfortable meeting for everybody,” the clinicians who had been involved with the Ayres

Clinic for years left en masse. This proved problematic for SII as the clinic had been a major

source of revenue; the majority of clients from

the clinic followed the clinicians, leaving no one

to run it and a limited clientele. Attempts to bring

in clinicians to bolster the clinic failed. In addition, courses began to falter; course quality was

seen to be diminishing; course attendees became

disgruntled as courses were scheduled and cancelled; the certifi cation process suffered; and

teaching faculty withdrew. In the end, Western

Psychological Services withdrew its support for

the certifi cation process, instead backing USC

and OT610, which was being offered through the

re-established clinic, Pediatric Therapy Network.

 In our interviews, we had talked with therapists who had been in the central core of Ayres’

infl uence and work, as well as those who were

increasingly more removed from her immediate sphere. Through these discourses we were

able to get a general sense of the process, some

insight into the impact on SI theory and ongoing

development of knowledge, but little agreement

as to exact timing or cause and effect relative to

unfolding tensions. Clearly, Ayres was aware of

developing discontent among the SII teaching

faculty. They were a group of independent thinkers, and this appeared to lead to multiple disagreements. We were told, “she knew she had not

prepared the fi eld for her leaving. And that when

she left, because of such strong belief systems, it

[teaching faculty] was gonna divide into at least

two factions. . . . [There was] bad blood between

them, and . . . nobody was willing to compromise

or see it as a research issue. . . . [Disagreements]

were about personalities and not about research,

and she didn ’ t know what to do.” Another interviewee stated an alternative impression, indicating that, having heard someone other than Ayres

conduct an SI lecture, Ayres responded, “I think

things are gonna be in good hands.”

 Perspectives on the directions taken after

Ayres’ death vary. We were told, “I think it gave

us all an extra impetus to carry on her legacy. . . .

And I think people really had a feeling that we

wanted to really carry on Jean ’ s legacy.” Another

interviewee indicated, “Different people took different pieces and tried to advance theory . . . and

practice. . . . That ’ s not a criticism. I think there

was a lot there that you could take and develop,”

indicating that individuals took “branches” of

the theory and practice to move them forward.

For instance, applying the theory to populations

of children other than those with learning disabilities was one “branch,” formalizing clinical

observations another, and specifi c prescribed

interventions (e.g., Therapressure Program) was

yet another.

 We wondered whether everyone saw growing

tension, and many people saw a “fray”; most

commented that they tried hard to stay out of it,

not wanting to take sides: “I tried to stay out of

it as much as possible”; “I saw it as somewhat

48 ■ PART I Theoretical Constructs

‘political.’” “I stayed out of what I saw as a

‘political mess.’” Generally, these individuals

did not necessarily see anything wrong with the

various directions people went. One interviewee

saw the work of Miller and colleagues in developing the Scientifi c Workgroup and encouraging

and facilitating research funding for colleagues

from a variety of disciplines as highly positive. “Lucy Jane Miller and that group that the

Wallace [Research] Foundation funded has had

tremendous impact on sensory integration . . .

people studying it from a wide variety of disciplines and publishing . . . would never have happened without an OT working within that group.

. . . Aligning ourselves with people like that

makes a big difference. . . . I think the intent

was to help to get us accepted by other professionals and disciplines.” However, other scholars

expressed concern that the different directions

would eventually lead to confusion. This concern

centered around a shift in terminology from

what had been “SI” to what Miller was calling

“sensory processing.”

 The use of different terminology was seen as

both positive and negative. On one hand, the shift

to sensory processing addressed the concern of

some that SI, as used by occupational therapists,

was not a term that was universally accepted and

understood . “So, by using a term that psychologists were more comfortable with, they had been

using [the phrase] sensory processing regulatory disorders already, . . . we would be more

accepted. I think that has actually happened.”

 Others saw it as a potential source of misunderstanding. The lack of clarity relative to defi ning SI and sensory processing was substantial.

Some scholars saw SI as the overall function, and

they included sensory processing as a component of SI. Other scholars saw the opposite, with

sensory processing as the umbrella term, within

which SI was a component. Unfortunately, this

conundrum continues to the present.

 One interviewee suggested that, with this

level of confusion, we would do best to dispense

with labels altogether, using descriptive language

instead:

 . . . the language is still confusing, haven ’ t

made much progress at all with language . . .

may be more useful to describe the behaviors that we see. For example, rather than the

term sensory modulation, let ’ s just say overreactivity to sensation, or under-reactivity. . . .

Let ’ s just describe the behavior and stop trying

to label it until we understand it better.

 We heard this call for a better understanding, for stronger science and research, and a

new wave of well-educated young scientists in

several interviews.

 Terminology was an ongoing issue in our

interviews. In part, the concern was regarding

use of the term sensory integration to describe an

intervention that we, in this book, have termed

sensory-based. We have included several sensorybased interventions in Chapter 18 (Complementary Programs for Intervention). Approaches such

as The Wilbarger Approach to Treating Sensory

Defensiveness, Therapeutic Listening®, and the

Astronaut Training Program have a foundation

in understanding how sensation is received, processed, and integrated to produce environmental

interaction, and here we include them as sensorybased. The treatment approaches described in

these programs are missing some of the essential

features of what is now called Ayres Sensory

Integration ® (ASI). In fact, the trademarked

symbol that now stands at the end of “Ayres

Sensory Integration” was a response to what

some saw as misappropriation of Ayres’ name

and work. Calling these approaches SI was seen

by some of our interviewees as problematic in

terms of theory development: “[this] has big consequences for the evolution of the theory, these

things calling themselves sensory integration, but

not being sensory integration.” It is also seen as

posing problems and confusion for teaching and

clinical practice.

 We get parents that say they want the brushing

protocol and we say, “No, we don ’ t do that,

but this is what we will do.” . . . It makes it

harder to teach some of the real SI kind of stuff

because people pull in . . . those kinds of things.

And, in some cases . . . when we ’ re teaching

. . . and we tell ‘em to come up with some treatment strategies and sometimes the fi rst thing is

brushing, and I think, “where did we miss out?”

And, so, it makes it harder. So, in some ways,

I think it can be harmful. It makes it harder for

. . . whoever ’ s trying to teach what we ’ re trying

to get across.

 Another point of ongoing contention has been

around the application of the phrase sensory

processing disorder. As the umbrella term in

the nosology ( Miller, Anzalone, Lane, Cermak,

& Osten, 2007 ), this phrase encompassed both

CHAPTER 3 Composing a Theory ■ 49

sensory modulation disorders and praxis disorders. However, as the term was applied to the

efforts toward inclusion in the Diagnostic and

Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth

Edition (DSM-5), the focus was on sensory modulation. The medical fi eld had, at least partially,

embraced the construct of regulatory sensory disorder ( Miller, Lane, Cermak, Anzalone, & Osten,

 2005 ), and many of the constructs of praxis were

already included in the developmental motor

coordination diagnostic category. Sensory processing disorder did not get accepted as a diagnosis in the DSM-5; however, “hyper-responsivity,”

“hypo-responsivity,” and “unusual interests

in sensory aspects of the environment” were

included in the criteria for autism ( American

Psychiatric Association, 2013 ). The DC: 0-5™

( Zero to Three, 2016 ) uses the term sensory

processing disorder to encompass sensory overand under-responsiveness, as well as sensory

seeking. As such, the issue with clarity on this

phrase remains, and it provides another point of

tension among SI scholars.

 I think when you ’ re talking about the language,

using the term sensory processing, which

is mostly about sensory modulation, versus

sensory integration, has confused people and led

them to believe that it ’ s all about modulation.

 Another commented,

 It ’ s kind of interesting to see how it all plays

out, but students don ’ t get it, and they think

that SI is just sensory modulation but then you

should add in some fi ne motor activities, and a

little bit of this and a little bit of that. But that

isn ’ t modulation; that is really looking at praxis,

and they don ’ t understand the praxis piece.

 [Ayres] had developed a SIPT, which was

a Sensory Integration and Praxis Test, and all

of a sudden the only thing we were talking in

the ’90s was about modulation . . . it was like

we had left some of the praxis out of the study

and everything was modulation . . . some of the

models on modulation were confusing for most

of the people. So, I think that we derailed for

many reasons.

 The tensions regarding developing branches

and terminology issues have not dissipated to

any substantial degree at the time of this writing.

Relative to “branches” we have tried to capture

those sensory-based therapeutic approaches

in Chapter 18 (Complementary Programs for

Intervention); we recognize that many of these

approaches have roots in Ayres’ theory of SI.

Further, we have decided in this book to use SI

to refl ect the theoretical and therapeutic foundation developed by Ayres, covering sensory modulation, sensory discrimination, and praxis; this

is refl ected in our model, presented in Chapter

1 (Sensory Integration: A. Jean Ayres’ Theory

Revisited). However, there are times when contributors preferred the term sensory processing

and we have left this terminology as is.

HERE ’ S THE POINT

• The CSSID (eventually renamed SII) was a

nonprofit organization formed to support SI

research via revenue generated from teaching.

• Ayres’ illness and withdrawal from both SII and

the Ayres Clinic resulted in a lack of leadership

and discord among members.

• Terminology around SI theory, intervention, and

classification of disorders has been an ongoing

point of contention among leaders in the fi eld.

Evolution of Ayres’ Work

 Through time, Ayres’ theory and models have

been revisited and adapted by others, as refl ected

in many of the comments noted previously.

These adaptations were fueled by several motivations. Some colleagues ( Fisher & Murray,

 1991 ) sought to capture and expand on SI in

ways consistent with Ayres’ writing but not specifi cally depicted in her models. Others (e.g., see

 Chapter 1 , Sensory Integration: A. Jean Ayres’

Theory Revisited; Miller, Anzalone, et al., 2007 )

sought to clarify links between sensory integrative processes and clinical fi ndings and behavior, sometimes in an attempt to simplify Ayres’

models and make them more readily available

outside the fi eld ( Miller, Anzalone, et al., 2007 ).

Still others (e.g., Dunn, 2014 ) focused on and

augmented particular aspects of Ayres’ theory.

In fact, so many new models have been created

that practitioners, and indeed researchers, often

seem confused about what constitutes SI theory.

Has one or more new theories emerged? Based

on our interviews and our review of various

published models, the answer to this question

is, decidedly, “ NO! ” As noted earlier, one of the

people we interviewed suggested “branches,”

and this analogy seems fi tting. Many of our SI

50 ■ PART I Theoretical Constructs

scholars have expanded their own work into particular areas of interest, extending the model of

SI and at times coupling it with other theoretical

models in their efforts to best serve children and

families. However, there remains a core; even as

available neuroscience knowledge has ballooned,

the inferences drawn and hypotheses put forward

by Ayres have been shown to be amazingly accurate. The core constructs of Ayres’ SI theory,

linking brain processing and observable behavior, have remained remarkably stable.

 We discuss some of the different published

models, endeavoring to show how they depict

the evolution of a single theory. As noted earlier,

Ayres’ original model was one of sensory integrative function. In her model, she illustrated the

ways in which the integration of various sensory

inputs contributed to different behaviors and performance skills, all in the end resulting in higher

level “end products.” None of the published

models contradicted these original relationships.

 We asked each of our interviewees to sketch

a model that depicted the way that they thought

about, or taught, SI theory, and none of those

deviated substantially from the relationships

that Ayres originally hypothesized. Instead, each

of the models expanded one or more aspects of

the original model. For instance, most further

defi ned “integration of sensory inputs” to include

both sensory discrimination and sensory modulation. Similarly, there was consensus that it is

the sensory discrimination aspect of integration

that supports the development of postural, ocular,

or bilateral skills; somatopraxis; and visuopraxis

(when this later construct is included). Sensory

modulation was generally seen to support behavior, attention, and arousal. Our scholars did not

always include the end products that Ayres had

included, but when they did, they were highlevel skills (e.g., academics, abstract thought).

Some of the scholars listed praxis as an end

product; others included praxis as a more basic

product of integration. It is safe to say that all

our interviewees would have added end products

had we specifi cally asked.

 At the STAR (Sensory Therapies And

Research) Institute, Miller and colleagues ( 2007 )

developed a model of practice grounded in ASI

theory that also includes core constructs from

specifi c sensory-based interventions and other

theories or models: caregiver-child interaction,

caregiver engagement in therapy, and caregiver

coaching and education. As noted earlier, one

of the skills Ayres brought to therapy was her

ability to help parents better understand children. However, this reframing was not explicit

in Ayres’ theory, which was focused on the

child rather than the family. Keep in mind that

Ayres’ work appeared during an era ensconced

in the medical model; SI theory was somewhat

more comprehensive than other theories of the

day in that it was closely tied to both evaluation and treatment. However, Ayres died before

family-centered care took hold, and she did

not formalize her concepts of family-centered

intervention.

 From a sensory integrative point of view, the

STAR Institute model emphasizes arousal regulation, linked, in many of our interviewees’

models, with sensory modulation, and focuses on

engagement and relationships. The STAR model,

then, does not refl ect a new “theory” but rather

the integration of theoretical models, linking SI

with engagement and relationships. More details

on the STAR model can be found in the Appendix to this text .

 Other models that will be familiar to many

practitioners are those developed by Dunn ( 1999,

2014 ). Dunn ’ s models focus on sensory modulation, one of the integrative functions. Dunn

expanded upon our understanding of the modulation defi cits that Ayres had originally identifi ed.

Ayres defi ned tactile defensiveness, postural/

gravitational insecurity, and aversiveness to

movement. Dunn, however, examined the interface between behavioral responses and neurological thresholds across sensory systems. Her

work expanded our understanding of differences

in the ability to modulate responses to sensation,

linking this to thresholds for activation within

the nervous system. Again, this was not a new

theory but rather a perspective on what underlies sensory responsivity. There is more information on Dunn ’ s model in Chapter 10 (Assessing

Sensory Integrative Dysfunction without the

SIPT), in the discussion of Dunn ’ s tool, the

Sensory Profi le 2.

 Since Ayres’ death, others have developed

interventions drawn in part from SI theory; we

have previously referred to many of these and

have captured several in Chapter 18 (Complementary Programs for Intervention). By and

large, they are sensory-based or regulationoriented. As with Dunn ’ s model, then, these other

CHAPTER 3 Composing a Theory ■ 51

interventions have branched from Ayres’ conceptualization of sensory modulation, and they do

not refl ect a new theory. Instead, they may be

best viewed as lines of thinking that grew from

the core of SI, making SI a theory with multiple expressions. Providing this expanded view

on aspects of Ayres’ work has moved our understanding forward in some areas. For instance, the

neurophysiological correlates of sensory overresponsivity (see Fig. 3-6 ) have been investigated

by several researchers (e.g., Lane, Reynolds, &

Thacker, 2010 ; Mangeot et al., 2001 ; McIntosh,

Miller, Shyu, & Dunn, 1999 ; Reynolds, Lane,

& Gennings, 2009 ; Schaaf et al, 2010 ; Schoen,

Miller, Brett-Green, & Nielsen, 2009 ), and added

to our understanding of this aspect of sensory

integrative dysfunction. More details on this

research can be found in Chapter 16 (Advances

in Sensory Integration Research: Basic Science

Research).

 Alongside of this increased knowledge base

regarding modulation, some of our interviewees expressed a fear that we had lost praxis:

one of the core components of SI theory. Ayres

completed the SIPT just before her death, and,

as such, the emphasis of the approach had been

on praxis. After her death, many researchers and

practitioners moved on to focus on modulation

and, as mentioned earlier, seemed to leave praxis

behind. Interviewees had this to say:

 It would be interesting to see where her research

would ’ ve gone . . . where would she be now

. . . when you look at the different eras . . .

there ’ s the era of the visual, more of the visual

motor. And with this space visualization and

fi gure ground tests coming out, and then vestibular becomes such a big deal, and then praxis

becomes such a big deal, and then sensory modulation or sensory registration becomes a big

deal, but it kind of stops there. . . . It feels like

we ’ re sort of stuck in that era of sensory modulation being the most important thing where in

the past it would be sort of like a 10-year period

where a lot of research was done and then we ’ d

move on to the next area, or Jean would move

on to the next area.

 I think it ’ s not just modulation. You certainly have to deal with modulation, but then

also people kind of have the idea [that] you

need to deal with the modulation fi rst, before

you can deal with the praxis, and don ’ t see it

[modulation] as something that just is all the

way through.

 This emphasis, this focus, had become a point

of disagreement for some of our interviewees,

potentially because it appeared that “sensory

processing” now meant “sensory modulation,”

which, in turn, is synonymous with SI. With this

chain of events, the place of praxis in SI (i.e.,

the impact of poor integration on the development of praxis) appears to have been lost. Given

that Ayres’ fi nal contribution to the fi eld was all

about praxis, this loss is disturbing. It is possible

that the apparent emphasis on sensory modulation was fueled by the growing understanding of

sensory modulation dysfunction in children with

autism, at a time when the diagnosis of autism

was on the rise. We were told:

 So, I think that we were caught in this also

because autism started being so prevalent, and

in the autism world . . . that was where the

money was. The money was in autism and . . .

into social and repetitive behaviors, and not so

much into praxis.

 This same interviewee went on to state: “the

evidence itself for the theory is more tipped to

modulation or to the social, emotional, repetitive

autism than the praxis, and it ’ s not like the praxis

is not present in the autism . . . but . . . it ’ s not

central in the diagnosis.”

Moving Forward

 Diversity in thinking is never a bad thing; it has

the potential to move things forward, uncover

alternative ways to accomplish tasks, and display

alternative ways to interpret situations ( Zollman,

 2010 ). Zollman explained that, as scientists, we

FIGURE 3-6 The Sensory Challenge Protocol,

developed by Miller, has been used to examine neural

underpinnings of sensory over-responsivity in various

populations of children.

52 ■ PART I Theoretical Constructs

actively seek information that guides us to determine the effectiveness of our methodologies.

Zollman argued for “transient diversity” (p. 32),

which can be understood as diversity of thought,

present for enough time that we carefully and

thoughtfully examine our own theories but not

so long that it interferes with the development of

a unifi ed direction. Diversity in thinking is consistent with the multiple directions and models

presented here. The unanswered question is

whether this diversity continues to benefi t the

broader fi eld, or if it is time to work toward

unifi cation.

 What have we gained from the diversity in

models and approaches; how have they moved

us forward? Certainly, we have gained a greater

understanding of the underpinnings of sensory

modulation. And perhaps we have developed

greater insight into the effect of specifi c sensory

and motor systems on behavior and development,

and gained perspectives on how to use sensation

in, and for, intervention. Some therapists might

also fi nd a renewed understanding of the nature

of family-centered and child-focused intervention. The various models and “branches” have

common roots in Ayres’ mission to understand

links among neurophysiological mechanisms,

sensory integrative dysfunction, clinical practice,

and occupational engagement and performance.

Whether these common roots are enough to work

toward unifi cation is itself a question that would

fuel debate.

 What hurdles will get in the way? Inconsistency in terminology is problematic; it confuses

the public; it confuses other professionals; and it

confuses our own practitioners. We are unlikely

to agree as to whether “SI,” “sensory processing,”

or “sensory integration and processing” is the

best descriptor. Nonetheless, our roots are in SI

theory, assessment, and intervention as developed

by Ayres. Use of the terms sensory processing

versus sensory modulation presents another

stumbling point although one, it seems, on which

we could come to consensus. Moving our understanding of praxis forward, and developing a

deeper understanding of sensory perception and

discrimination and their links to praxis, is crucial.

It is important to recognize that research funding

is increasingly diffi cult to obtain, and this area

of research will be no exception. However, other

investigators within and outside of occupational

therapy are engaged in research on interventions

for motor incoordination; the longer we wait, the

less likely SI theory is to be linked with praxis.

 There are other considerations as we look to

models and their application in practice. Parham

and colleagues ( 2007 ) identifi ed the core elements of sensory integrative therapy in the ASI

Fidelity Measure ( Parham et al., 2007, 2011 ).

This tool, based on the fi delity measure developed by Miller, Coll, and Schoen ( 2007 ), guides

practice and research to maintain “fi delity” to the

core principles of ASI. Although some research

has been conducted on the effectiveness of the

intervention, more needs to be done, and funding

will be needed to accomplish this research.

 Schaaf and Mailloux ’ s ( 2015 ) clinical reasoning

approach, focused on using assessment fi ndings

to guide the choice of goals and defi ning clear

outcomes when using SI in treatment, has merit

but has not been thoroughly investigated. In this

text we have addressed SI theory as a basis for

coaching (Chapter 17, Using Sensory Integration

Theory in Coaching) as a tool to enable children

to succeed in everyday tasks despite sensory

integrative dysfunction. Clearly integrating and

evaluating the effectiveness of task and environment interventions within models of SI intervention also requires investigation.

HERE ’ S THE POINT

• New conceptual models and models of

intervention have emerged based upon Ayres’

work, but they largely align with her original

theory.

• Since Ayres’ death, much research has emerged

in the area of sensory modulation; however,

more focused study is needed in the areas of

sensory discrimination and praxis.

• Continued research is needed on the effect of

interventions that have their roots in SI theory.

Summary and Conclusions

 Conducting these interviews shed important

light on the history of SI theory and practice.

Our colleagues refl ected on their understanding

of SI along many different paths, and by listening and considering their comments, we came to

realize there was a base of positive intent that

ran through each perspective. The interviews

pointed to more similarities than differences. The

CHAPTER 3 Composing a Theory ■ 53

stories we heard were heart-warming and heartwrenching. Only one scholar we wished to

include in these interviews refused to talk to us.

We gained a better picture of Ayres as a person,

a scholar, a researcher, a clinician, a theoretician,

and a reluctant leader. We heard remarkably little

disparity on how history unfolded. Although relevant scholars were not fully unifi ed in modeling

SI, the differences seemed more to refl ect different emphases rather than schisms in the theory.

Nonetheless, there remains much to do, much to

learn, and much to understand as we continue to

study and apply SI theory to practice.

Acknowledgments

 Although we decided not to identify individual

contributions to the information presented in this

chapter, we do want to express our sincere thanks

to all who we interviewed as we pursued these

historical insights. To Erna Blanche, Sharon

Cermak, Florence Clark, Dottie Ecker, Diana

Henry, Judy Kimball, Sue Knox, Lawrene Kovalenko, Zoe Mailloux, Shay McAtee, Lucy Miller,

Shelley Mulligan, Diane Parham, Roseann

Schaaf, Sarah Schoen, Susanne Smith Roley, and

Pat Wilbarger: We thank you all for your knowledge and wisdom, your willingness to share, and

your time in interviews, reviews of transcripts,

and review of this chapter. To say the chapter

would not have been possible without you is a

signifi cant understatement.

Where Can I Find More?

 Here you will fi nd some of Ayres’ early work.

You might enjoy reading this history for yourself!

 Ayres, A. J. (1954). Ontogenetic principles in

the development of arm and hand functions.

American Journal of Occupational Therapy,

8 (3), 95–121.

 Ayres, A. J. (1958). The visual-motor function.

American Journal of Occupational Therapy,

12 (3), 130–138.

 Ayres, A. J. (1961). Development of the body

scheme in children. American Journal of

Occupational Therapy, 15 (3), 99–102.

 Ayres, A. J. (1963). The development of

perceptual-motor abilities: A theoretical basis

for treatment of dysfunction. American Journal

of Occupational Therapy, 17, 221–225.

 Ayres, A. J. (1964). Tactile functions. Their

relation to hyperactive and perceptual motor

behavior. American Journal of Occupational

Therapy, 18, 6–11.

 Ayres, A. J. (1965). Patterns of perceptualmotor dysfunction in children: A factor analytic study. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 20,

 335–368.

 Ayres, A. J. (1966a). Interrelation of perception, function, and treatment. Journal of the

American Physical Therapy Association, 46,

741–744.

 Ayres, A. J. (1966b). Interrelationships among

perceptual-motor abilities in a group of

normal children. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 20 (6), 288–292.

 Ayres, A. J. (1966c). Interrelationships among

perceptual-motor functions in children. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 20 (2),

68–71.

 Ayres, A. J. (1969a). Defi cits in sensory integration in educationally handicapped children. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 2 (3),

160–168.

 Ayres, A. J. (1969b). Relation between Gesell

developmental quotients and later perceptualmotor performance. American Journal of

Occupational Therapy, 23 (1), 11–17.

 Ayres, A. J. (1971). Characteristics of types of sensory integrative dysfunction. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 25 (7), 329–334.

 Ayres, A. J. (1972a). Basic concepts of occupational therapy for children with perceptualmotor dysfunction. In Proceedings of the

Twelfth World Congress of Rehabilitation

International, pp. 154–161.

 Ayres, A. J. (1972b). Improving academic scores

through sensory integration. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 5 (6), 23–28.

 Ayres, A. J. (1977). Cluster analyses of measures

of sensory integration. American Journal of

Occupational Therapy, 31 (6), 362–366.

 Ayres, A. J. (1978). Learning disabilities and the

vestibular system. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 11 (1), 30–41.

 Ayres, A. J., & Mailloux, Z. (1981). Infl uence of

sensory integration procedures on language

development (aphasia, apraxia, vestibular

disorder). American Journal of Occupational

Therapy, 35 (6), 383–390.

 Ayres, A. J., & Tickle, L. S. (1980). Hyperresponsivity to touch and vestibular stimuli

54 ■ PART I Theoretical Constructs

as a predictor of positive response to sensory

integration procedures by autistic children.

American Journal of Occupational Therapy,

34 (6), 375–381.

 Cermak, S. A., & Ayres, A. J. (1984). Crossing

the body midline in learning-disabled and

normal children. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 38 (1), 35–39.

 Sieg, K. W. (1988). Six perspectives on theory

for the practice of occupational therapy.

 Salem, MA: Aspen Publishers, Inc.

 Here are some additional reads that you might

fi nd interesting:

 1. Ayres, A. J. (2004). Love, Jean: Inspiration

for families living with dysfunction of

sensory integration. Santa Rosa, CA:

Crestport Press.

 A compilation of letters written by Ayres

to her nephew, Philip Erwin, that provides

unique insights into her thoughts about her

research in SI and her own sensory needs.

 2. Henderson, A., Llorens, L., Gilfoyle,

E., Myers, C., & Prevel, S. (1974). The

development of Sensory Integrative Theory

and Practice: A collection of the works of

A. Jean Ayres. Dubuque, IA: Kendall/Hunt

Publishing Company.

 A collection of scholarly works written by

Ayres that outline the development and

refi nement of SI theory and practice.

 3. Cermak, S. A., Ayres, A. J., Coleman, G.,

Smith Roley, S., Mailloux, Z., & McAtee, S.

(2011). Ayres dyspraxia monograph, 25th

anniversary edition. Seattle, WA: Amazon

Digital Services LLC.

 Original work by Ayres that provides

a foundation for understanding the

neurobiological basis for praxis and

dyspraxia, with additional material provided

by current clinicians and scholars.

 References

 American Psychiatric Association . ( 2013 ). Diagnostic

and statistical manual of mental disorders

( 5th ed. ). Washington, DC : Author .

 Ayres , A. J. ( 1949 ). An analysis of crafts in the

treatment of electroshock patients. American

Journal of Occupational Therapy , 3 , 195 – 198 .

 Ayres , A. J. ( 1955a ). Proprioceptive facilitation

elicited through the upper extremities. American

Journal of Occupational Therapy , 9 ( 1 ), 1 – 9 , 50 .

 Ayres , A. J. ( 1955b ). Proprioceptive facilitation

elicited through the upper extremities. American

Journal of Occupational Therapy , 9 ( 2 ), 57 – 77 .

 Ayres , A. J. ( 1955c ). Proprioceptive facilitation

elicited through the upper extremities. American

Journal of Occupational Therapy , 9 ( 3 ), 121 – 144 .

 Ayres , A. J. ( 1972 ). Sensory integration and learning

disorders . Torrance, CA : Western Psychological

Services.

 Ayres , A. J. ( 1989 ). Sensory Integration and Praxis

Test (SIPT) manual . Torrance, CA : Western

Psychological Services.

 Ayres , A. J. ( 2004 ). Love, Jean: Inspiration for

families living with dysfunction of sensory

integration . Santa Rosa, CA : Crestport Press .

 Ballinger , G. A. ( 2007 ). Individual reactions to

leadership succession in workgroups. Academy of

Management Review , 32 ( 1 ), 118 – 136 .

 Dunn , W. ( 1999 ). Sensory Profi le . San Antonio, TX :

 The Psychological Corporation .

 Dunn , W. ( 2014 ). Sensory Profi le-2: User ’ s manual .

 San Antonio, TX : The Psychological Corporation .

 Fisher , A. G. , & Murray , E. ( 1991 ). Introduction

to sensory integration theory . In A. G. Fisher ,

 E. A. Murray , & A. C. Bundy ( Eds .), Sensory

integration theory and practice ( pp . 3 – 26 ).

 Philadelphia, PA : F. A. Davis .

 Gorman , M. E. , & Kashani , N. ( 2017 ). A. Jean Ayres

and the development of sensory integration: A

case study in the development and fragmentation

of a scientifi c therapy network . Social

Epistemology , 31 ( 2 ), 107 – 129 . doi:10.1080/02691

728.2016.1241322

 Henderson , A. , Llorens , L. , Gilfoyle , E. , Myers , C. ,


& Prevel , S. ( 1974 ). The development of sensory

integration theory and practice . Dubuque, IA :

 Kendall/Hunt Publishing Company .

 Kielhofner , G. ( 2004 ). Conceptual foundations of

occupational therapy . Philadelphia, PA : F. A.

Davis Company .

 Lane , S. J. , Reynolds , S. , & Thacker , L. ( 2010 ).

 Sensory over-responsivity and ADHD:

Differentiating using electrodermal responses,

cortisol, and anxiety . Frontiers in Integrative

Neuroscience , 4 ( 8 ), 1 – 14 . doi:10.3389/

fnint.2010.00008

 Mangeot , S. D. , Miller , L. J. , McIntosh , D. N. ,

 McGrath-Clarke , J. , Simon , J. , Hagerman , R. J. ,

& Goldson , E. ( 2001 ). Sensory modulation

dysfunction in children with attention-defi cithyperactivity disorder . Developmental Medicine

and Child Neurology , 43 , 399 – 406 .

 May-Benson , T. A. , & Koomar , J. A. ( 2010 ).

 Systematic review of the research evidence

examining the effectiveness of interventions

using a sensory integrative approach for children .

American Journal of Occupational Therapy , 64 ,

 403 – 414 . doi:10.5014/ajot.2010.09071

 McIntosh , D. N. , Miller , L. J. , Shyu , V. , & Dunn , W.

( 1999 ). Overview of the Short Sensory Profi le

(SSP) . In W. Dunn ( Ed .), The Sensory Profi le:

CHAPTER 3 Composing a Theory ■ 55

Examiner ’ s manual ( pp . 59 – 73 ). San Antonio, TX :

 The Psychological Corporation .

 Miller , L. J. , Anzalone , M. E. , Lane , S. J. , Cermak ,

 S. A. , & Osten , E. T. ( 2007 ). Concept evolution

in sensory integration: A proposed nosology for

diagnosis. American Journal of Occupational

Therapy , 61 ( 2 ), 135 – 140 .

 Miller , L. J. , Coll , J. R. , & Schoen , S. A. ( 2007 ).

 A randomized controlled pilot study of the

effectiveness of occupational therapy for children

with sensory modulation disorder . American

Journal of Occupational Therapy , 61 , 228 – 238 .

 Miller , L. J. , Lane , S. J. , Cermak , S. , Anzalone ,

 M. , & Osten , E. ( 2005 ). Regulatory-sensory

processing disorders. In S. I. Greenspan & S.

 Weider ( Eds .), ICDL diagnostic manual for

infancy and early childhood ( pp . 73 – 112 ).

 Bethesda, MD: ICDL .

 Northouse , P. A. ( 2015 ). Leadership theory and

practice ( 7th ed. ). Los Angeles, CA : SAGE

Publications, Inc .

 Parham , L. D. , Cohn , E. S. , Spitzer , S. , Koomar ,

 J. A. , Miller , L. J. , Burke , J. P. , . . . Summers , C.

( 2007 ). Fidelity in sensory integration intervention

research. American Journal of Occupational

Therapy , 61 , 216 – 227 .

 Parham , L. D. , Roley , S. S. , May-Benson , T. A. ,

 Koomar , J. , Brett-Green , B. , Burke , J. P. , . . .

 Schaaf , R. C. ( 2011 ). Development of a fi delity

measure for research on the effectiveness of the

Ayres Sensory Integration intervention . American

Journal of Occupational Therapy , 65 , 133 – 142 .

 doi:10.5014/ajot.2011.000745

 Reynolds , S. , Lane , S. J. , & Gennings , C. ( 2009 ).

 The moderating role of sensory over-responsivity

in HPA activity: A pilot study with children

diagnosed with ADHD. Journal of Attention

Disorders , 13 , 468 – 478 .

 Schaaf , R. C. , Benevides , T. , Blanche , E. I. , BrettGreen, B. A. , Burke , J. P. , Cohn , E. S. , . . .

 Schoen , S. A. ( 2010 ). Parasympathetic functions

in children with sensory processing disorder .

Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience , 4 , 4 .

 doi:10.3389/fnint.2010.00004

 Schaaf , R. C. , & Mailloux , Z. ( 2015 ). Clinicians

guide for implementing Ayres Sensory

Integration ® . Bethesda, MD : AOTA Press .

 Schaaf , R. C. , Schoen , S. A. , May-Benson , T. A. ,

 Parham , L. D. , Lane , S. J. , Smith Roley , S. ,

& Mailloux , Z. ( 2015 ). The issue is—State of

the science: A roadmap for research in sensory

integration. American Journal of Occupational

Therapy , 69 , 6906360010 . doi:10.5014/

ajot.2015.019539

 Schoen , S. A. , Miller , L. J. , Brett-Green , B. A. ,

& Nielsen , D. M. ( 2009 ). Physiological and

behavioral differences in sensory processing: A

comparison of children with autism spectrum

disorder and sensory modulation disorder .

Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience , 3 , 1 – 11 .

 doi:10.3389/neuro.07029.2009

 Sieg , K. W. ( 1988 ). Six perspectives on theory for

the practice of occupational therapy . Salem, MA :

 Aspen Publishers, Inc .

 Zero to Three . ( 2016 ). DC: 0–5™ Diagnostic

classifi cation of mental health and developmental

disorders of infancy and early childhood .

 Washington, DC : Author .

 Zollman , K. J. S. ( 2010 ). The epistemic benefi t

of transient diversity . Erkenntnis , 72 , 17 – 35 .

 doi:10.1007/s10670-009-9194-6


PART

II

The Neuroscience

Basis of Sensory

Integration Disorders

58

CHAPTER

4

Structure and Function

of the Sensory Systems

 Shelly J. Lane , PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA

 Chapter 4

 . . . the structure/function duality is merely a didactic convenience. In reality,

structure allows function and function gives meaning to structure.

 — Cohen, 1999 , p. 3

Upon completion of this chapter, the reader will be able to:

✔ Identify components of the central and

peripheral nervous systems.

✔ Review the structure and function of the

primary sensory systems associated with sensory

integration theory.

✔ Develop an understanding of links between

sensory system structure and sensory integrative

function and dysfunction.

LEARNING OUTCOMES

Purpose and Scope

 Students faced with their fi rst neuroanatomy

class commonly feel a sense of foreboding.

There is seemingly endless detail within the

central nervous system (CNS) and the connecting peripheral nervous system (PNS), and

developing a thorough understanding not only

of structure and function but also of interrelationships between and among structures and

functions seems a daunting undertaking. In this

chapter, the learning process is a little less daunting: A small piece of the neuroscience pie is presented, beginning with some basics relative to

the central, peripheral, and autonomic nervous

systems, and then emphasizing sensory system

structure, function, and integration. We will

address in most detail the sensory systems most

closely aligned with sensory integration (SI)

theory, the tactile and proprioceptive (together

referred to as somatosensory ), as well as vestibular, systems. Auditory and visual systems

will be covered in somewhat less depth, and

the olfactory and gustatory systems will also be

presented. A single chapter on only the sensory

systems can hardly do justice to these topics;

entire books have been written about each. Thus,

this should be considered an overview, a review

for many readers. For each system, an attempt

has been made to provide integrative information

that combines structure and function. For more

detailed information, readers are referred to the

books listed in the reference list and to the other

chapters of this book.

Basic Structure and Function

of the Central Nervous System

 Before diving into the structure and function of

the sensory systems, we need to consider where

this information fi ts in SI theory. In Chapter 1

(Sensory Integration: A. Jean Ayres’ Theory

Revisited), a schematic of sensory integrative

dysfunction was presented, and it is reproduced

here, adapted to include “interoception” ( Fig. 4-1 ).

CHAPTER 4 Structure and Function of the Sensory Systems ■ 59

As previously described, the fi gure is best understood by fi rst considering the sensory systems,

located at the center of the schematic. Understanding the sensory systems is central to understanding SI theory; this is where we begin.

Cells of the Central Nervous System

 The basic building block of the nervous system

is the neuron. The neuron consists of a cell body,

axons, and dendrites ( Fig. 4-2 ). The cell body

is the metabolic center of the neuron. Extending from the cell body are two types of processes: axons and dendrites. Typically, there is

only one axon, which carries information from

the cell body to the target. Although generally

only a single axon exits a cell body, it may split

into many branches, thereby allowing a single

neuron to infl uence many targets. Axon diameter varies from 0.2 to 20 μ m, a feature that will

help determine the speed with which information

is transmitted; the larger the axons, the more

rapid the transmission. Axons may be myelinated or surrounded by a nodular sheath of fatty

substance. Myelin offers insulation to the nerve

fi ber and increases transmission speed along the

axon. Axons act similar to cables in a communication network, carrying information within

and across regions of the CNS. And, similar

to cables, larger axons with more insulation

(myelin) transmit information more quickly

and with less loss of signal strength than do

the small, uninsulated axons ( Bear, Connors, &

Paradiso, 2015 ).

 Dendrites are responsible for bringing information into the cell body. Dendrites are often

extensively branched, allowing communication

with many other neurons. The dendritic branches

are rich in synapses, allowing for immense

communication with other neuronal fi bers. This

structure is foundational for the integration of

inputs, a basic function of the nervous system

( Bear et al., 2015 ). Dendrites and axons combine

to form the pathways of the CNS. Fiber bundles

and pathways travel varying distances within the

CNS and carry information from the CNS to the

effector organs and muscles in the body.

 Glial cells surround and greatly outnumber

neurons. One form of glia, the astrocyte, is shown

in Figure 4-3 . Glial cells do not have the same

FIGURE 4-1 Complex schematic representation of sensory integrative dysfunction. In this version of the model,

we have added interoception as a sensory input.

Autonomic Limbic Reticular Thalamus Cerebellum Basal Ganglia Cortex

Behavioral

consequences

Indicators of

poor sensory

modulation

 Over-

 responsivity

• Aversive

 and

 defensive

 reactions

 Under-

 responsivity

 • Poor

 registration

Inadequate

CNS integration

and processing

of sensation

Visual

Vestibular

Tactile

[lnteroception]

Auditory

Olfactory

Gustatory

Proprioception

Indicators of poor sensory

integration and praxis

Poor

postural-ocular

control

 Poor sensory

 discrimination

• Tactile

• Proprioception

• Vestibular

• Visual

• Auditory

Poor body

schema

Sensory reactivity

Sensory perception

VBIS

Behavioral

consequences

Poor selfefficacy,

self-esteem

Sensory

seeking

Poor

organization

Poor gross,

fine, and

visual motor

coordination

Avoidance of

engagement

in motor

activities

Clowning

Occupational Engagement Challenges

Occupational Engagement Challenges

Sensoryrelated

challenges

with attention,

regulation,

affect, activity

Somatodyspraxia

Poor selfefficacy,

self-esteem

Withdrawal

from, and

avoidance of,

sensory

experiences

Sensory

seeking

60 ■ PART II The Neuroscience Basis of Sensory Integration Disorders

electrical transmission properties of neurons, but

instead they:

• Provide structural support to the nervous

system.

• Insulate groups of neurons from each other.

• Remove debris after injury or cell death.

• Buffer the electrochemical environment in

which neurons exist.

• Nourish neurons.

• Act as stem cells, able to give rise to new

glial cells and possibly new neurons.

HERE ’ S THE POINT

 • There are multiple resources to support

learning in neuroscience, and they tend to

take different approaches to the topic. Readers

may need to access more than one resource to

develop a thorough understanding of structures

and functions in the nervous system.

 • The basic building block of the nervous system

is the neuron, consisting of a metabolic center

(cell body), input (dendrite) and output (axon)

fi bers.

Central and Peripheral Nervous

System Structure

 The CNS and PNS are the anatomical divisions of our nervous system; the brain, spinal

cord, and meninges form the CNS, whereas

the cranial and spinal nerves make up the PNS.

There are neuronal cell bodies, axons, and dendrites in both divisions. The nervous system can

also be characterized functionally as autonomic

(ANS) and somatic. The ANS consists of central

FIGURE 4-2 The neuron, with a myelin sheath around the axon. From Thompson, G.S. [2013]. Understanding

Anatomy and Physiology [1st ed.]. Philadelphia: F.A. Davis, p. 157, with permission.

Nucleus

The cell body (also called the soma) is the

control center of the neuron and contains the

nucleus.

Gaps in the myelin sheath, called nodes of

Ranvier, occur at evenly spaced intervals.

The end of the axon branches extensively, with

each axon terminal ending in a synaptic knob.

Within the synaptic knobs are vesicles containing

a neurotransmitter.

Dendrites, which look like the bare branches of a

tree, receive signals from other neurons and

conduct the information to the cell body. Some

neurons have only one dendrite; others have

thousands.

The axon, which carries nerve signals away from

the body, is longer than the dendrites and contains

few branches. Nerve cells have only one axon;

however, the length of the fiber can range from a

few millimeters to as much as a meter.

The axons of many (but not all) neurons are

encased in a myelin sheath. Consisting mostly of

lipid, myelin acts to insulate the axon. In the

peripheral nervous system, Schwann cells form

the myelin sheath. In the CNS, oligodendrocytes

assume this role.

CHAPTER 4 Structure and Function of the Sensory Systems ■ 61

FIGURE 4-3 Glial cells in the CNS take different forms. In A, oligodendrocytes form myelin sheaths around

axons. B. Microglia are macrophages, the immune cells of the brain and spinal cord. C. Astrocytes provide

metabolic support for neurons and help regulate neural transmission. D. Ependymal cells line the ventricles in

the brain and the central canal of the spinal cord, serving as a brain and cerebral spinal fl uid interface.

Oligodendrocyte A B

C D

Microglia

Astrocytes

Myelin

sheath

Neuron

Neuron

Neuron

Neuron

Capillary

Cilia

Ependymal cells

structures and nerve processes that innervate

smooth muscle and glands, and the somatic

nervous system consists of the components that

carry neural signals to or from muscles, joints,

and skin (Siegel, Sapru, & Siegel, 2015). These

aspects of the nervous system are shown in

 Figure 4-4 . Simplistically, the human nervous

system can be likened to a communications

network with central hubs and projections transmitting messages into and out of the central core

(i.e., the CNS).

 Beginning with structural organization, the

PNS is composed of receptors, or specialized

nerve endings, and the neurons that conduct

information to and from the CNS. The PNS then

connects the outside world and peripheral structures (e.g., skeletal muscles and glands) to the

brain and spinal cord.

 Each sensory system has unique and specialized receptor cells or nerve endings that are

particularly sensitive to one form of physical

energy; for instance, rods and cones in the retina

are specialized receptor cells that respond to light

energy, whereas the Pacinian corpuscle in the

skin is a specialized nerve ending that responds

to deep pressure and vibration. Once a receptor responds to a sensory stimulus, a cascade of

events takes place that results in transmission of

the specifi c features of that input over afferent

fi bers to the CNS. Features of sensation that are

sent to the CNS include the characteristics such

as the intensity of the input, the duration, and,

of course, the kind of sensation ( Purves et al.,

 2011 ). The PNS also has efferent nerve fi bers

and specialized receptors that transmit the signal

from the CNS to effectors. When the effectors

are muscle fi bers, the receptor is the neuromuscular junction. Effectors may also be visceral

structures, innervated by fi bers associated with

the ANS. Here, too, there will be specialized

receptors.

 The ANS mediates homeostasis; it regulates

such things as blood pressure, heart rate, respiration, and digestion by responding to pressure and

stretch in organs and glands, changes in body

chemistry, pain, and temperature ( Purves et al,

 2011 ; Siegel & Sapru, 2013 ). The use of the label

“autonomic” indicates that this component of

62 ■ PART II The Neuroscience Basis of Sensory Integration Disorders

FIGURE 4-4 Central (pink), peripheral (light pink), and autonomic (black and dark pink) nervous system

distribution. The ANS is further broken out into parasympathetic (black) and sympathetic (dark pink)

components. Note that most internal organs and glands receive both parasympathetic and sympathetic

innervation. From Eagle, S., et al. (2009). The professional medical assistant. Philadelphia, PA: F.A. Davis

Company: p. 438; with permission.

Cerebrum

Cerebellum

Brainstem

Spinal cord

To arm

and head

To trachea

and lung

To heart

To kidney

To liver

and spleen

To intestines

To thigh To colon

and rectum To knee,

lower leg,

and foot

Central nervous

system

Peripheral nervous

system

Autonomic nervous

system

Parasympathetic

system

Sympathetic

system

To bladder

To external

genitalia

To eye

To glands

in head

To stomach

CHAPTER 4 Structure and Function of the Sensory Systems ■ 63

the nervous system functions without conscious

control in maintaining the body ’ s physiological

homeostasis. Information received is transmitted

to the CNS through peripheral and cranial nerves.

Within the CNS, the hypothalamus, thalamus,

and limbic system, along with areas within the

medulla and pons, are responsible for mediating

autonomic functions. The efferent fi bers of this

system innervate smooth muscle, cardiac muscle,

and glandular epithelium.

 The efferent component of the ANS is composed of two major divisions: sympathetic and

parasympathetic. The sympathetic division functions to prepare the body for fi ght or fl ight; it is

most active during periods of stress and serves to

increase the body ’ s use of energy. The parasympathetic division has a “rest and digest” function, restoring the energy stores in the body by

promoting digestion of food and absorption of

nutrients. These two major divisions of the ANS

may innervate the same organ and act in concert

to continuously regulate activity in that organ.

For example, the heart is infl uenced by both

sympathetic and parasympathetic inputs that

control functions such as heart rate. Sympathetic

activation will increase heart rate whereas parasympathetic activation will decrease heart rate.

The systems act in concert to mediate cardiac

response to changes in the internal or external

environment.

 The enteric nervous system is an additional

aspect of the ANS that functions in a semiindependent manner ( Fig. 4-4 ) ( Purves et al.,

 2011 ). This system innervates the gastrointestinal

(GI) tract, carrying both sympathetic and parasympathetic information. Interestingly, there are

many neurons in the GI system that are not under

ANS control, yet they are still part of the enteric

nervous system; this is why it is considered

“semi-independent.” The enteric nervous system

monitors and controls chemical and mechanistic

aspects of GI function.

 We will talk more about interoception, but

we will not further address the specifi cs of ANS

structure or function in this text. However, the

way in which sensory processing takes place

within the nervous system is often interpreted as

a refl ection of ANS activity. For instance, when

a child with tactile defensiveness overreacts to a

nudge from a classmate, the sympathetic component of the ANS may have been activated. Similarly, when we suggest the use of deep pressure

or heavy work to act as a calming or focusing

agent for a child, we are considering the potential ability of this input to increase activity in

the parasympathetic division of the ANS. Thus,

as you read, consider how the behaviors identifi ed in children or their responses to intervention

potentially refl ect activity within the ANS. We

have pointed out when systems project to components of the ANS.

Central Nervous System Geography

 The CNS consists of the brain and the spinal

cord. The spinal cord contains both afferent and

efferent fi bers carrying information to and from

the brain and cell bodies located in the PNS.

In addition, there are numerous local interneurons (small neurons that reside entirely within

the cord) that are responsible for information

processing and integration. Using a computer

analogy, the spinal cord is roughly equivalent

to a large housing through which several cables

run from peripherals to the computer tower.

However, because processing does occur in the

spinal cord, this analogy falls somewhat short.

 The brain can be grossly divided into the cerebrum or cerebral hemispheres, diencephalon, cerebellum, and brainstem ( Fig. 4-5 ). The cerebrum

includes four lobes with which you are likely

familiar (i.e., frontal, parietal, occipital, temporal; see Fig. 4-6 ) and two others, the limbic lobe

or cingulate gyrus (visible on the medial surface

of the brain) and the insular lobe (forming the

fl oor of the lateral fi ssure). The diencephalon

is composed of the thalamus, hypothalamus,

epithalamus, and subthalamus. The brainstem

is made up of the pons, medulla, and midbrain

( Fig. 4-5 ).

 The midbrain is also referred to as the mesencephalon, a refl ection of terminology used

in describing embryological brain development.

Within the midbrain are the inferior and superior

colliculi, associated with the auditory and visual

systems, respectively; together they are referred

to as the tectum, forming a tent over the cerebral

aqueduct. As an aside, the superior colliculus (SC)

is also considered an important integration center

for sensory input because it receives input from

multiple sensory systems ( McHaffi e, FuentesSantamaria, Alvarado, Gutierrez-Ospina, & Stein,

 2012 ). Another midbrain region, the periaqueductal gray, surrounds the region of the cerebral

64 ■ PART II The Neuroscience Basis of Sensory Integration Disorders

FIGURE 4-5 Two regions of the diencephalon, the thalamus and the hypothalamus, are shown here. Gylys &

Wedding: Medical Terminology Systems, A body systems approach, 6e. F.A. Davis, Philadelphia, with permission.

Cerebrum

Diencephalon

(interbrain)

Thalamus

Hypothalamus

Pons Medulla

Midbrain

(mesencephalon)

Cerebellum

Spinal cord

aqueduct and thus is located adjacent to the

tectum. In the subsequent sections of the chapter,

we discuss each of these CNS components as

they relate to the sensory systems.

 Brodmann areas are also referred to throughout this chapter. Brodmann areas represent a

numbering system of brain regions developed by

Brodmann in 1909. He thought that each of the

52 numbered regions defi ned a discrete histological unit within the brain. Subsequent work has

shown that only some of these areas have clear

functions. However, the numbering that has survived is a useful reference point for identifying

cortical regions. Brodmann areas are shown in

 Figure 4-7 .

Central Nervous System Function

 Organization of function within the CNS was once

thought to be strictly hierarchical, with increasing complexity in the interpretation of input and

planning of output as information moved from

the spinal cord to the cerebral cortex. According

to Squire and colleagues ( 2012 ), this hierarchical

organization exists and is very apparent in the

motor system. In addition, there is a hierarchy in

FIGURE 4-6 Lobes of the brain.

(1) Frontal lobe

(2) Parietal lobe

(4) Occipital lobe

(3) Temporal lobe

CHAPTER 4 Structure and Function of the Sensory Systems ■ 65

processing sensory input such that at each level

within the CNS, greater specifi city in interpretation of input is attained. However, the complexity of interactions within and between CNS

levels indicates that a heterarchical or network

organization also exists. Sensory information

reaches all levels within the motor systems, and

motor system output is infl uenced not only by

sensory input but also by cognitive processing,

other intrinsic activity (e.g., sleep–wake cycles,

behavioral state, arousal level, motivation), and

sensory feedback from ongoing motor activity.

Thus, organization of the CNS is best considered as complex, embracing some aspects of a

hierarchy and other interactions more consistent

with the intense interaction found in a network of

linkages ( Squire et al., 2012 ).

 Terminology

 Some functional considerations become important as we begin to study the CNS. Because they

are all at work in all sensory systems, we defi ne

them here and then refer to them within each

system as appropriate.

Stimulus Reception and Transduction

 Receptors within each system respond optimally

to specifi c types of sensory input. Thus, specifi c

tactile pressure receptors respond best to touch or

pressure, and photoreceptors in the eye respond

best to light. Although the receptors are different for each system, the process of changing

the input from physical to electrochemical has

some similarities. Using the tactile system as an

example, deep pressure activates a receptor, such

as the Pacinian corpuscle. The pressure changes

membrane characteristics in the receptor, and

this process leads to transduction of the mechanical (pressure) input into an electrical signal.

From physiology class, you may recall that all

cells have an “electrical potential” established by

the distribution of charged ions on the inside and

outside of the cell membrane. Activation of the

receptor (in this case, touch pressure) leads to

changes in the distribution of these ions, which

changes the charge distribution across the membrane and results in a local depolarization of the

immediately surrounding membrane. The term

applied to this local depolarization is a receptor

potential ( Fig. 4-8 ). At times, when the stimulus

is very weak, the electrical charges are minimal

and the receptor potentials are not strong enough

to lead to transmission beyond this level, so the

input is transmitted only a short distance from

where it began, similar to a whisper spoken to a

large group of people. Those close by may hear

it, but unless they pass it on, those on the far side

of the group will not, and the message will die a

short distance from where it began.

 If the stimulus is of suffi cient intensity or

applied for a long enough period of time, receptor potentials can be added together, and the

result is the production of an action potential

in the sensory neuron. An action potential is

also a change in the distribution of charge on

the membrane, but it is strong enough to depolarize neighboring areas on the neuron, and a

wave of depolarization is generated that carries

the information to its fi rst synapse on the way to

the CNS.

FIGURE 4-8 Receptor potential; local change in ion

distribution that, if of suffi cient intensity, will transmit

information from the receptor toward the CNS.

Reprinted with permission from Clinical

Neuroanatomy, 26th edition, by Stephen Waxman.

Copyright McGraw-Hill Education.

FIGURE 4-7 Brodmann areas on the lateral surface

of the brain.

1

2 3

5 7

8 6

4

46

45 44

47

9

10

11

40

39

19

37

20

22

52

21

43 41 42

38

18

17

66 ■ PART II The Neuroscience Basis of Sensory Integration Disorders

Stimulus Encoding

 One action potential, in any system, generated by

any input is the same as another. How, then, does

the CNS discriminate between bright light and

fi rm touch? Discrimination relies on the specifi city of receptors for a type of sensory input and

requires interpretation within the CNS, based

on pathways and connections of the sensory

neurons. Receptors convey the information that

a touch was fi rm instead of soft by encoding the

stimulus characteristics into a pattern of action

potentials that represents intensity, duration, and

movement of the stimulus. A stronger stimulus

results in an increase in the frequency of action

potentials sent to the CNS and is likely to activate more receptors when applied. Thus, strong

inputs are read as such in the CNS because

they generate more action potentials within one

neuron and because the input is detected by

multiple receptors so that action potentials are

transmitted by a large number of neighboring

neurons. This process of specifi city in receptors

has parallels in computer systems as well. The

electrical wires that connect both the keyboard

and mouse to your computer are essentially the

same; they transmit electrical current in the same

manner after the current is generated. The specifi city comes from the receptors, in this case the

keyboard keys and mouse pad. The keys respond

to pressing, and they respond best when we press

them in an appropriate sequence, as in spelling a

word. The mouse responds to a different input, a

dragging touch that moves around the mouse pad.

We can change the frequency (hitting a keyboard

key multiple times) or infl uence the “intensity”

of the input or word we are typing by doing such

things as typing in bold or in all capitals.

Receptor Fields

 The term receptor fi eld or receptive fi eld

refers to the area around a receptor from which

input can be transduced into an electrical signal

( Fig. 4-9 ). This concept is applied to mechanical receptors of the tactile system where the

receptor fi eld is that area of skin surrounding a

single receptor, which activates the receptor. In

the visual system, the receptor fi eld of a photoreceptor is that area of the retina in which it is

found. Small receptor fi elds are associated with

fi ne discriminative function because they contribute to a precise representation of input at the

CNS. Returning to the computer analogy, this is

similar to the receptive area available on a standard keyboard compared with the key pad on a

smartphone.

Receptor Adaptation

 Receptors adapt to continued input, and depolarization of the receptor membrane ceases, even

with continued input. Some receptors are considered rapid adapting, responding only at the

onset and offset of input. Others are slow adapting, responding in a more continual manner to

ongoing input, but eventually even these will

cease to produce action potentials. The adaptation of receptors becomes critical in the function of the sensory systems, and this function

plays a role in providing ongoing information

about what is happening (slow adapting receptors), along with information on changes in the

internal or external environment (rapid adapting

receptors).

Lateral Inhibition

 Lateral inhibition ( Fig. 4-10 ) is another phenomenon that is important to understanding how

we receive and interpret neural signals. It is the

mechanism used by the CNS to focus input from

the receptors and thereby sharpen its interpretation. Lateral inhibition relies on the presence

of inhibitory interneurons. It happens like this:

a stimulus—for example, a touch—is applied

to the hand. Receptors in the skin are activated,

FIGURE 4-9 Large and small receptive fi elds. Note

that with smaller receptive fi elds the same stimulus

(in this image, the two red arrows) activates two

neurons, whereas with larger receptive fi elds only a

single neuron is activated. The smaller receptive fi eld

transmits more detailed information about what has

touched the skin.

Region with small

receptive fields

Large RF

low resolution

Small RF

high resolution

Region with large

receptive fields

No

response

Neural firing

from one

receptive field

Neural firing

from both

receptive fields

CHAPTER 4 Structure and Function of the Sensory Systems ■ 67

and those with their receptive fi elds centered

under the stimulus respond with greater strength

(more action potentials and more rapid fi ring).

This is analogous to speaking softly to a group

of people; those directly in front of the speaker

will hear the bulk of the message, but those on

the edges will capture less of the information.

Fibers from the receptor will synapse with other

sensory neurons as they transmit information to

the CNS, much as the people in this group talk

with their neighbors. In the absence of lateral

inhibition, the activation pattern spreads widely,

with increasing numbers of sensory neurons activated, and the result at the CNS is some general

awareness that a large region of the skin was

touched. In the speaking example here, it would

be as though each person who heard the message

spoke it again to a neighbor, and the neighbor

shared it with another neighbor so that soon

the whole room would be buzzing with sound.

However, the specifi cs of that sound would be

rather vague. Lateral inhibition is used to focus

the input rather than allow it to be diffused over

many neurons. In sensory systems using this

focusing mechanism, the neurons at the center of

the receptor fi eld (i.e., those most intensely activated by the input) activate inhibitory interneurons at their fi rst synapse within the CNS. These

inhibitory interneurons connect with sensory

neurons farther from the center of the receptive

fi eld (i.e., less active neurons) and inhibit transmission at the periphery of the receptive fi eld.

Other terms used to defi ne this process include

surround inhibition and inhibitory surround.

 The process is the equivalent of people in the

center of a room gently shushing their neighbors

to prevent them from speaking. This cuts down

on the background noise in the room, making the

original message more clearly delivered, at least

to the people permitted to hear it. In the CNS,

lateral inhibition serves to focus the input at each

relay station, reducing background noise. This

results in the ability to discriminate and localize the input we receive. Sensory systems with

well-developed discriminatory functions rely on

this mechanism.

Convergence and Divergence

 Convergence and divergence ( Fig. 4-11 ) are

concepts that need to be understood in order

to appreciate the accuracy with which information is conveyed from the PNS to the CNS.

With convergence, many cell processes synapse

at one site. Thus, many axons may synapse on

the same neuronal cell body or dendrite. When

this happens, a great deal of information is condensed. This can be useful for increasing the

intensity of the information in the CNS and for

promoting integration, but the tradeoff is that

the specifi city of the original input is decreased,

similar to several members of an audience offering input to the speaker in rapid succession. The

speaker can listen and integrate the information,

but the specifi cs of where each bit of information

came from, exactly what was said and by whom,

are likely to be lost.

 Divergence, on the other hand, occurs when

one process synapses with many different cells

in the CNS. Earlier in this chapter, we mentioned that axons can divide and infl uence many

other cells; divergence is such an example. An

axon leaves a cell body and subsequently makes

contact with cell bodies or dendritic branches in

FIGURE 4-10 Schematic of lateral inhibition. In

this diagram, the stimulus is applied as shown, and

the receptors directly under it will respond with full

strength, whereas those located laterally will respond

with less strength. Thus, receptor B will respond at

100%, A and C at 80%, and D at 60%. The small

inhibitory interneurons reduce the stimulus by 20%.

Thus, stimulus strength in each neuron is reduced

by 40% at this level, and neurons A and C now

transmit at only 40%, B at 60%, and D at 20%. This

information transmitted from this point forth then is

more focused; little information is transmitted over

neuron A and none over neuron D. This process can

occur at all synapses along the route of transmission,

and it will serve to sharpen the initial input received

by the receptor. (The values associated with stimulus

strength and interneuron inhibition are arbitrary.)

Stimulus

Receptors:

receive stimulus

at strength related

to location

A

Inhibitory

interneurons:

Each inhibits

strength of signal

by 20%

BC D

68 ■ PART II The Neuroscience Basis of Sensory Integration Disorders

several other areas of the CNS. The functional

association here is that the same information is

represented at many places, repeated over and

over. Thus, the potential impact of this information can be widespread.

Habituation and Sensitization

 Our neural networks are highly malleable;

changes in synaptic connectivity can be both

short or long duration. One of the goals of occupational therapy using SI is to impact neural

connectivity through the long term. Two mechanisms have been linked with long-term changes:

habituation and sensitization. Behaviorally, you

are likely well aware of habituation; you put on

clothes in the morning, adjust them to feel comfortable, and then quickly forget about them.

This is habituation, a process of adapting to

sensory input, of becoming less responsive to

repeated sensation. Sensitization occurs when an

aversive response to a specifi c stimulus is generalized to other, previously non-aversive stimuli.

For instance, a child contentedly eats lunch in

the school cafeteria until one day when a cart

with 20 trays topples, causing a very loud crash

and instant chaos. Greatly startled by the combination of the crash and the chaos, the child now

pairs these inputs with the cafeteria environment

and refuses to go there for lunch. The effect can

be either short- or long-lasting, depending on

other aspects of CNS processing.

Distributed Processing and Control

 The CNS can be masterful in its capacity to

“multitask.” You read this chapter while you

maintain your posture, shift weight to relieve

uncomfortable pressure, possibly eat and drink,

and digest whatever it is you are drinking or

eating. All activities are organized and directed

by the CNS. You are engaging sensory and

motor systems, cognitive processes, and autonomic functions simultaneously via distributed

processing within the CNS. When it works well,

distributed processing allows for effi cient and

effective interactions with the world because

the load is distributed among different centers

of control.

Serial and Parallel Processing. Underlying distributed control of activities are two processing

methods: serial and parallel. In serial processing, things occur in sequence, one after another,

in a hierarchical manner. Transmitting information from a touch receptor to the CNS occurs this

way as the information is mechanically received,

transduced into an electrical signal, summed to

form an action potential, and transmitted to the

CNS. Parallel processing involves the work of

more than one pathway working simultaneously.

Visual, vestibular, and proprioceptive systems

often use parallel processing to orient us to our

position in space. Each system processes different bits of information about our bodies and

FIGURE 4-11 A schematic depicting convergence and divergence in the CNS.

Reprinted with permission from Lundy-Ekman, L. Neuroscience: Fundamentals for Re-habilitation , p. 37.

Philadelphia, PA: WB Saunders Co., 1998.

Convergence Divergence

Principles of nerve centers

coordination

CHAPTER 4 Structure and Function of the Sensory Systems ■ 69

the space around us with the integrated endpoint

being maintenance of upright posture.

Parallel processing is a term also used when

the same sensation is carried and processed in

two pathways that have some redundancy. For

instance, pain input is processed via both the

dorsal column medial lemniscal (DCML) and the

spinothalamic pathways. This functional overlap

can be very useful in the face of disease or dysfunction that interrupts the fl ow of information in

one system. The fact that the same information

can be processed in a parallel system can be capitalized upon in intervention.

 There are other processes and mechanisms

that could be discussed in this background

section, but the ones offered here provide a very

basic anatomic and functional baseline. We now

focus on the individual sensory systems and integration among sensory systems as a basis for

understanding their contributions to SI and occupational performance.

HERE ’ S THE POINT

 • The nervous system can be conceptualized

as consisting of anatomic divisions (CNS and

PNS) or functional divisions (ANS and somatic).

Together these approaches provide insight into

structure and function.

 • The PNS connects our periphery and

environment to the CNS, bringing sensory

input to the brain (afferent input) and carrying

central commands away from the brain to the

muscles and organs (efferent output).

 • Similar to the PNS, the ANS has afferent and

efferent components; these components

mediate physiological homeostasis.

 • The enteric nervous system functions in a

semi-independent way to mediate functions of

the gut.

 • Organization of the CNS, comprising the brain

and spinal cord, is best considered a complex

interaction of hierarchical and heterarchical

structure and function confi gurations.

 • Knowledge of common neuroscience-related

terminology supports an understanding of

structure, function, and dysfunction in the

nervous system.

The Somatosensory System

 We begin our discussion of the somatosensory

system by reviewing the receptors associated

with somatosensation. After being gathered by

the receptors, somatosensory inputs are processed over the DCML pathway, spinocerebellar

pathways, and the pathways of the anterolateral system (AL). We present each separately.

We also discuss the trigeminothalamic pathway,

which is responsible for the transmission of

somatosensory information from the face. A brief

description of the functional overlap between

the somatosensory pathways completes this

section.

Receptors and Transduction

 The majority of receptors in the somatosensory

system are mechanoreceptors. This means that

when a mechanical force (e.g., light touch, deep

pressure, stretch, or vibration) is applied to the

receptors, the process of neural transmission

begins. Proprioceptive input from joints and

PRACTICE WISDOM

Why do we care about things such as receptor

types, receptive fi eld, speed of transmission,

and neuronal functions such as lateral inhibition, as well as processes such as divergence,

convergence, and habituation? Understanding these functions helps us understand what

we are doing clinically when we make sensory

experiences available to the children we treat.

For instance, if a child brushes lightly against

a fuzzy surface and this triggers a defensive or

withdrawal response, we likely have triggered

a hair cell receptor. We know that these receptors are connected to small neural fi bers, and

the information will travel to the CNS relatively

slowly. When we use fi rm pressure to rub the

spot where the fuzzy surface touched, we are

activating deeper receptors, such as the Merkel

disc or Pacinian corpuscle. These receptors are

connected to larger diameter fi bers that are

myelinated. This information travels faster to the

CNS, and we can use it in effect to diminish the

effect of light touch on orienting and arousal. So

even though you might not be thinking about

receptor types and speed of transmission, you

are likely using these constructs clinically and in

everyday life.

70 ■ PART II The Neuroscience Basis of Sensory Integration Disorders

TABLE 4-1 Locations, Modalities of Sensation, Adaptation Rates, and Fiber Types Associated with

Skin Receptors

TYPE LOCATION STIMULUS FIBER TYPE ADAPTATION

Free nerve ending Dermis, joint capsules,

tendons, ligaments

Pain, temperature A-delta, C Slow

Hair follicle plexus Deep dermis Hair displacement; pain A-beta Fast

Meissner corpuscles

(tactile corpuscles)

Papillae of skin, mucous

membranes of tongue tip

Touch A-beta Fast

Pacinian corpuscles Subcutaneous tissue Pressure, vibration A-beta Fast

Krause end bulb Papillae of hairless skin;

near hair follicle plexus

Cold A-delta, C Below 20°C;

no adaptation

Merkel disc Epidermis of hairless skin;

hair follicles

Deformation of skin A-beta Slow

Ruffi ni ending Joint capsules; connective

tissue

Touch; skin stretch;

joint movement

A-beta Slow

muscles is certainly a mechanical input, and proprioceptive input is carried along somatosensory

pathways. Although we discuss proprioception

briefl y here, readers will also fi nd proprioception covered in conjunction with the vestibular

system in other chapters of this book. The tactile

system also includes thermoreceptors and is thus

responsible for the interpretation of temperature

input. Table 4-1 lists the receptors within this

system and their various characteristics.

Tactile Mechanoreceptors

 Mechanoreceptors within the skin subserve different types of sensory activation. Some respond

to the initiation and cessation of input (Meissner

corpuscle, Pacinian corpuscle, some hair follicles) but not to sustained input. These receptors

are considered fast adapting because they stop

responding to maintained stimuli. They are sensitive to changes in tactile input. Slowly adapting

receptors include Merkel discs, Ruffi ni endings,

and some hair follicle receptors. In contrast to

the fast-adapting receptors, this second group

of receptors provides the CNS with information

regarding the intensity, duration, and speed of

input ( Fig. 4-12 ).

 Our tactile discrimination ability depends, in

part, on the density of receptors and the associated size of the receptor fi eld. In areas of fi ne

tactile discrimination (e.g., fi ngertips, palms,

around the mouth), receptor density is high and

the receptor fi eld is small. Areas of high receptor

density are more skilled. However, in areas where

less specifi c information is needed about tactile

input (e.g., abdomen and back), receptor density

is low and receptor fi elds are larger. Researchers

(e.g., Jones & Smith, 2014 ) examining discrimination abilities have shown that, although some

things can be distinguished using either active

or passive touch, the use of active touch allows

fi ner and more accurate discrimination. The

tactile system also capitalizes on lateral inhibition to focus input and refi ne discrimination.

 The somatosensory system, which carries

information from the body to the CNS, has two

main subdivisions: the DCML system and the AL

pathways; there are also crucial projections of

proprioception to the cerebellum. The pathways

will be described later in this section. Together,

the information received by receptors and transmitted over these pathways provides us with the

ability to interpret our tactile world and respond

appropriately to touch. Because of the pervasive nature of these two somatosensory subdivisions, they are critical to our interactions with

the world. Likewise, the pervasive nature of the

somatosensory system means that when problems exist, the impact can be widespread.

Proprioception and Proprioceptors

 Sherrington ( 1906 ) defi ned proprioception as

perception of joint and body movements as well as

position of the body, or body segments, in space;

this defi nition is consistent with what we read

CHAPTER 4 Structure and Function of the Sensory Systems ■ 71

today in the literature (e.g., Croker, 2013 ; Proske

& Gandevia, 2012 ) and most neuroscience texts

( Bear et al., 2015 ; Purves et al., 2011 ; Siegel,

Sapru, & Siegel, 2015). Proprioceptors provide

a continuous fl ow of sensation from muscles,

joints, and tendons, and they inform us about

the spatial orientation of the body or body parts,

the rate and timing of movements, the amount

of force our muscles are exerting, and how

much and how fast a muscle is being stretched

( Proske & Gandevia, 2012 ). The continuous fl ow

of information is essential; imagine how it would

be to have only intermittent information about

your limb and body position in and movement

through space. Although Sherrington ( 1906 ) and

others identifi ed muscle afferents, joint receptors,

and the vestibular labyrinth as proprioceptors,

we will confi ne the discussion in this section to

the non-vestibular proprioceptors.

 Before the early 1970s, researchers distinguished between conscious joint proprioception

(kinesthesia), thought to arise primarily from

joint receptors, and unconscious proprioception,

thought to arise from the muscle spindle and

tendon receptors. However, this distinction is not

clearly made, and older experimental evidence

indicates that all proprioceptive inputs can contribute to conscious proprioception ( Matthews,

 1988 ; McCloskey, 1985 ; McCloskey, Cross,

Honner, & Potter, 1983 ; Moberg, 1983 ; Tracey,

 1985 ).

 For purposes of studying SI theory, it is more

important to understand the distinction between

proprioceptors (i.e., proprioceptive receptors)

and proprioception (i.e., proprioceptive feedback

and perception of joint and body movement). Not

all proprioception is derived from peripheral proprioceptive receptors. Internal correlates of the

motor signals that are sent to the muscles after

an action is planned are also an important source

of proprioception; the process is termed corollary. Corollary discharge happens when the plan

for movement is sent to the cerebellum; the cerebellum “previews” the plan, and it makes some

updates based on current information it receives

from the muscles and joints. It helps us to fi nesse

FIGURE 4-12 Skin touch receptors include free nerve endings (dermis and superfi cial epidermis), Merkel disc

and Meissner corpuscle (dermis), and Ruffi ni ending or corpuscle and Pacinian corpuscle (subcutaneous tissue).

Free nerve

endings

(temperature

receptor)

Merkel disc

(touch receptor)

Ruffini corpuscle

(pressure receptor) Meissner corpuscle

(touch receptor)

Pacinian corpuscle

(pressure receptor)

Subcutaneous

tissue

Dermis

Epidermis

Free nerve endings

(pain receptor)

72 ■ PART II The Neuroscience Basis of Sensory Integration Disorders

our motor actions. Corollary discharge is important for differentiating between active (internally

generated) and passive movement (generated

by an external stimulus), identifying if we have

programmed an appropriate level of motor

activity, the development of body scheme, and

perception of force ( Crapse & Sommer, 2008 ).

Knowledge of the body and its movements is

important in motor planning and is also addressed

in Chapter 5 (Praxis and Dyspraxia).

Sources of Proprioceptive Input. Proprioceptive feedback arises from several sources.

Muscle spindles, located in the muscle and

running parallel to muscle fi bers ( Fig. 4-13 ), are

the primary source of proprioception, providing

input relative to joint position and movement

through the midrange of movement. Proske and

Gandevia ( 2012 ) indicated that there is indirect

evidence for a contribution from Golgi tendon

organs (GTO) relative to weight and force. The

GTO is located in the muscle tendon. Proprioception from joint receptors is minimal and primarily at the end range of movement ( Proske

& Gandevia, 2012 ), serving primarily as limit

detectors ( Ferrell & Smith, 1988 ).

 Looking more closely at how the muscle

spindle functions, the effective stimulus for the

primary and secondary endings of this receptor is stretch. Primary fi bers transmit information regarding the velocity of change in muscle

length, as well as amount of change, and secondary fi bers transmit information on static positions

and sustained stretch and contraction. Both types

of fi bers are critical for ongoing information

about the location of the body and limbs in space.

 Information about muscle stretch travels to

the spinal cord, where the sensory input connects with ascending fi ber pathways (discussed

later) and locally with the alpha motor neuron

of the same muscle. Alpha motor neurons are

efferent fi bers coming from the spinal cord and

going to muscle fi bers to produce muscle contraction. One alpha motor neuron may connect

with several muscle fi bers; the name for the

alpha motor neuron and all the muscle fi bers to

which it connects is a motor unit. Stretch of the

muscle activates the muscle spindle, which, in

turn, activates the alpha motor neuron, leading

to muscle contraction. Muscle contraction leads

to increased muscle tension and activation of the

GTO, a tension-sensitive receptor.

FIGURE 4-13 The muscle spindle with afferent (sensory) fi bers carrying proprioception to the CNS, and efferent

(motor) fi bers to muscle (extrafusal fi ber) and muscle spindle (intrafusal fi ber). In the tendon there is the Golgi

tendon organ, responding to muscle tension.

Motor neuron

to extrafusal

muscle fiber

Motor neuron

to intrafusal

muscle fiber Muscle

spindle

Tendon

organ Sensory

axon

Muscle spindle connective

tissue capsule

Sensory axons

Sensory nerve endings

Sensory

nerve endings

Intrafusal

muscle fibers

Extrafusal muscle fibers

Tendon organ capsule (connective tissue)

To CNS

From CNS

CHAPTER 4 Structure and Function of the Sensory Systems ■ 73

 Stretching or contracting a muscle a little

bit results in a little bit of proprioceptive input;

working against resistance recruits motor units,

providing more information to the CNS about

body and limb position in and movement

through space ( Schmidt & Lee, 2011 ). This is

a concept we can use clinically. For example,

when we extend the head and upper trunk against

gravity from the prone-lying position, extend

weight-bearing limbs to jump on a trampoline, or

fl ex our arms while swinging on a suspended trapeze, we are contracting against the resistance of

gravity and body weight. In contracting against

resistance, we recruit motor units to produce the

needed force for the activity. Therefore, evincing

an adaptive behavior against resistance may be

the most effective means available for generating

proprioceptive feedback.

 Stimulation of cutaneous or skin mechanoreceptors and joint receptors by active joint movement is believed to be particularly important in

the perception of movement in some, but not

all, body areas. For instance, loss of cutaneous

input during movement of the knee has not been

shown to impair the ability to determine joint

position (see Gokeler et al., 2011 for review),

and loss of one source of proprioception can be

compensated for by input from other receptors

( Bear et al., 2015 ).

 Although tactile and proprioceptive information travel in the same pathway (see the text that

follows), it is important not to confuse cutaneousgenerated proprioception with tactile sensation.

Proprioception refers to sensations of movement

or position that arise because of an individual ’ s own movement. Tactile sensation pertains

to awareness or perception of the location, or

change in position, of an external stimulus

applied to the skin. Tactile sensation provides an

individual with information about the external

environment acting on the skin and skin receptors. Often, tactile information is gathered from

movement of joints; this can be a bit confusing. However, by defi nition, inputs such as deep

touch pressure and passive joint compression are

not sources of proprioception.

 As an example, consider a child swinging on

a swing. Proprioceptors are activated by body

movements such as holding onto the swing,

pumping with the legs, and leaning forward and

back with the trunk. Touch receptors are activated in the hand as it feels the rope handles as

well as on the head and body as air fl ows around

the movement and the child ’ s hair moves from

one direction to the next. Both sources of input

provide information about position in and movement through space. The vestibular system plays

a role here as well, something that we will come

back to soon.

 Centrally generated motor commands and

efference copy are also sources of proprioceptive feedback. They are thought to be responsible

for the sense of effort or conscious awareness

that proprioception is happening ( Schmidt &

Lee, 2011 ). According to the classic work of

 McCloskey ( 1985 )

 We have all experienced the sensation of

increasing heaviness of a suitcase which we

carry with progressively fatiguing muscles.

Ultimately, we put down such a load and rest

when it has “become too heavy.” But the load

has not really become heavier: the pressure

and tensions in the supporting limbs have not

increased, and there is no reason to assume that

the discharges from sensory receptors signaling

pressures or tensions will have increased either.

What makes the load seem heavier is that one

perceives the greater effort, the greater efferent barrage of voluntarily-generated command

signals, which has been necessary to maintain

a contraction with progressively fatiguing and

so less responsive muscles. Similar sensations of heaviness or increased muscular force

accompany all other states of muscular weakness whether caused experimentally . . . or by

disease. (p. 152)

 Centrally-generated motor commands and

efference copy from motor centers are speculated to be necessary for accurate interpretation

of sensation ( Schmidt & Lee, 2011 ). Centrallygenerated motor commands and efference copy

are also important in motor control, that is, in

the planning and producing of an adaptive motor

behavior. We present more information on these

concepts in Chapter 5 (Praxis and Dyspraxia).

Dorsal Column Medial Lemniscal

(DCML) Pathway

 Receptors associated with the DCML respond

to mechanical stimuli, transmitting primarily

tactile, vibratory, touch-pressure, and proprioceptive information. The DCML is associated

with functions inherent to tactile discrimination

or perception: detection of size, form, contour,

74 ■ PART II The Neuroscience Basis of Sensory Integration Disorders

texture, and movement across the skin. Because

it carries proprioceptive information, the DCML

also transmits information relative to the position

of the body and limbs in space.

 Inputs are transduced into a set of action

potentials and transmitted over the axon to the

cell body, which, in this case, is in the dorsal root

ganglion. There is no synapse here, and the information is passed from the dorsal root ganglion cell

body to dendrites that enter the spinal cord and

travel to the brain via the dorsal columns of the

spinal cord. The DCML is shown in Figure 4-14 .

The fi rst synapse of the DCML is in the medulla,

in the gracile and cuneate nuclei.

 From the medulla, fi ber tracks cross and form

the medial lemniscal fi bers, traveling through the

brainstem reticular formation and ascending to

the ventral posterior lateral (VPL) nucleus of the

thalamus. The fact that fi ber tracks cross in the

brain rather than in the spinal cord has functional

implications in the face of an injury or dysfunction. If there is a problem with this pathway at

or above the medulla, the functional loss will

be on the opposite side of the body. If there is a

problem within this pathway below the medulla,

it will be refl ected on the same side of the body.

 Fibers enter the thalamus, synapse, and then

send third-order neurons to the cortex. Cortical reception areas for the DCML include the

primary and secondary somatic sensory cortex

(S-I and S-II, respectively; Fig. 4-15 ), as well as

Brodmann areas 5 and 7 of the posterior parietal lobe, the somatosensory association cortex

( Figs. 4-7 and 4-15 ). The processing up to this

point has been an example of hierarchical processing, with more refi ned information processed

at each level. Within the cortex, the hierarchy is

less obvious.

 In S-I, the somatosensory receptor density and

location are precisely represented in a somewhat

distorted image of the body known as the sensory

homunculus ( Fig. 4-15 ). Interestingly, it has

been shown that this representation of the body

at the cortical level is fl exible. Classic research

has shown that areas representing specifi c body

parts can be increased in size with intense use,

and, likewise, representation is decreased with

disuse and concomitant diminished skill ( Coq &

Xerri, 1999 ; Jenkins, Merzenich, Ochs, Allard,

& Guic-Robles, 1990 ; Mogliner et al., 1993 ;

 Recanzone, Merzenich, & Jenkins, 1992 ). These

fi ndings are important to occupational therapy

intervention in general and sensory integrative

intervention specifi cally.

 Processing throughout the DCML promotes

its discriminative functions. The somatotopic

organization of the fi bers is precise, with fi bers

from the leg and foot taking a medial position.

As fi bers representing the upper leg, trunk, and

upper extremity enter the cord, they are added

to the pathway laterally. The relationship of

the fi bers to each other is maintained with high

integrity as they travel through the CNS. The

somatotopic organization of the fi ber pathways is

also maintained in the medullary nuclei and in

the pathway as it ascends. However, the pathway

twists as it approaches the thalamus so that fi bers

from the arm come to lie medial to those from

the leg.

 Precise somatotopic organization is only one

reason that information in the DCML is transmitted with great accuracy. Other reasons include:

• A minimal number of relays where the signal

must be processed

• Little convergence of input en route to

the CNS

• Heavy reliance on lateral inhibition to

maintain the integrity of a stimulus from the

periphery to the CNS

 These features allow the brain to interpret the

temporal and spatial aspects of DCML inputs,

yielding a great deal of information about the

location and type of somatosensory information

received ( Abraira & Ginty, 2013 ; Bear et al.,

 2015 ).

Interpreting Somatosensory Input

 Although we generally consider interpretation

of sensation to be a function of the cortex, or at

least higher levels of the CNS, some processing begins within the gracile and cuneate nuclei

in the medulla for the DCML. In addition to

somatosensation, these nuclei receive input from

the primary sensory cortex and the reticular formation. This convergence of input means that

activity in the primary sensory cortex, as well as

the reticular formation, infl uences the interpretation of tactile input, even before it reaches a

cortical level.

 Thalamic interpretation of DCML inputs is

thought to permit vague conscious discrimination of tactile input. There exist inhibitory

CHAPTER 4 Structure and Function of the Sensory Systems ■ 75

FIGURE 4-14 Dorsal column medial lemniscal system. Note that the information transmitted over this pathway

comes from muscle spindles, skin, and joint receptors. That from the lower extremity is transmitted to the

nucleus gracilis and that from the upper extremity to the nucleus cuneatus. Reprinted with permission from

Gilman, S., and Newman, S. W. Essentials of Clinical Neuroanatomy and Neurophysiology , 9th edition, p. 62.

Philadelphia, PA: F.A. Davis Co., 1996.

76 ■ PART II The Neuroscience Basis of Sensory Integration Disorders

FIGURE 4-15 The primary sensory cortex (S-I) and secondary sensory cortex (S-II) are shown in fi gure A. Within

S-I is the sensory homunculus (fi gure B). From Kandel, ER, and Jessell, TM: Touch. In Kandel, ER, Schwartz, JH

and Jessel, TM: Principles of Neural Science, ed 3. Appleton and Lange, Norwalk, CT, 1991 with permission,

pp 368 [A] and 372 [B].

Foot

Trunk

Hip

Leg

Shoulder

Head

Neck

Forearm

Elbow

Arm

Waist

Hand

Little finger

Ring

Middle

Index

Thumb

Eye

Nose

Face

Upper lip

Lower lip

Teeth, gums, and jaw

Tongue

Pharynx

Intra-abdominal

Toes

Genitals

A

B

interneurons in the VPL that are activated by

fi bers from the cortex as well as inhibited by

fi bers from other thalamic nuclei. Cortical activation of inhibitory interneurons interferes with

further transmission of DCML inputs beyond the

thalamus. On the other hand, if the inhibitory

interneurons are themselves inhibited by other

thalamic projections, information can be processed and sent onto cortical areas. Thus, there

is some processing at this level that infl uences

transmission beyond the thalamus.

 The primary somatosensory cortex (S-I, Brodmann areas 3, 1, and 2; Figs. 4-7 and 4-15 ) is

subdivided into different processing areas associated with differing types of sensation. Area 3a

receives a great deal of input from the thalamus

but primarily related to body position in space as

opposed to touch. Area 3b is considered to be the

primary somatosensory processing region because

neurons here respond only to somatosensation

(touch and proprioception) ( Bear et al., 2015 ).

Considerable somatosensory information passes

through 3b before going to areas 1 and 2. Projections to area 2, from area 3b, relate to size and

shape. Area 2 also receives signifi cant amounts

of information from muscle spindles and GTOs,

CHAPTER 4 Structure and Function of the Sensory Systems ■ 77

giving it an important role in proprioception and

kinesthesia. Loss of texture discrimination has

been associated with damage to area 1, and loss

of stereognosis has been associated with damage

to area 2. Area 3b has been associated with losses

of both of these functional skills, as 3b processes

information before passing it on to the areas 1 and

2. This means that areas 1 and 2 likely elaborate

sensory input and are, therefore, associated with

higher level interpretation of information.

 The secondary sensory cortex (S-II, Brodmann

area 43; Figs. 4-7 and 4-15 ) receives input from

the VPL as well as from S-I. However, without

a functioning primary sensory cortex, neurons

within the secondary cortex do not fi re. Thus, the

secondary cortex depends on the primary cortex

for input. Within the S-II, new sensory discriminations are thought to take place. Projections

from the secondary cortex to the insular lobe

are believed to be involved in tactile memory

( Purves et al., 2011 ).

 Further interpretation of somatosensory

inputs takes place in the somatosensory association cortex, areas 5 and 7 of the parietal lobe

( Figs. 4-7 and 4-15 ). These regions not only

receive input from the thalamus but also from

S-I and S-II, and they are connected bilaterally.

Some images show these areas to be part of S-II

in that they are projection areas from the primary

sensory cortex. Both of these parietal regions

play sensory integrating roles: area 5 for touch

and proprioception, and area 7 for somatosensory and visual inputs. Such processing of inputs

from multiple sources makes this a good example

of heterarchical organization within the CNS.

Because of these connections, lesions in areas

5 and 7 result in defi cits in spatial perception,

visual-motor integration, and directed attention.

Both areas are also associated with the manipulation of objects and are important in discerning

their tactile qualities (i.e., haptic perception). Any

clinician who has tried to evaluate stereognosis

in children who do not automatically manipulate

objects can appreciate the importance of manipulation for tactile perception. Lesions in these areas

within the right hemisphere have been associated

with agnosia of the contralateral side of the body

and body space. Tactile sensation is not impaired,

but individuals fail to recognize and attend to this

side of the body and the environment around it.

 Also, within the parietal lobe, aspects of

tactile and proprioceptive input converge and

subsequently project to anterior motor planning

areas of the brain. Thus, output from the DCML

could be expected to have an impact on both

object manipulation and motor planning. In fact,

when outputs from area 2 (a subregion of S-I) to

the primary motor cortex are disrupted, hand use

becomes uncoordinated. A decrease in sensory

feedback to the motor cortex that occurs secondary to interruption of the DCML interferes with

the production of coordinated fi ne motor acts.

 Proprioceptive input has been linked with

arousal ( Rose, Ahmad, Thaller, & Zoghbi, 2009 ),

and inputs from the DCML may have a role in

arousal modulation. Clinically, certain types of

sensory information have been observed to have

a calming effect clinically. Deep touch pressure

and proprioceptive information have been theorized, and shown by some, to have this quality

( Ayres, 1972 ; Chen, Yang, Chi, & Chen, 2013 ;

 Edelson, Edelson, Kerr, & Grandin, 1999 ;

 Reynolds, Lane, & Mullen, 2015 ; Vasa et al.,

 2014 ); both are carried to the CNS via the dorsal

columns.

 Clinicians and researchers have indicated that

poor tactile perception may be related to diffi -

culties in manipulative hand skills ( Goodwin &

Wheat, 2004 ; Haron & Henderson, 1985 ; Johansson & Flanagan, 2009 ; Yu, Hinojosa, Howe, &

Voelbel, 2012 ). Furthermore, diffi culty in perceiving the size and form of an object during the

process of active manipulation results in diffi -

culty handling the object. We may also speculate

that diffi culty in perceiving the boundaries of the

hand and the relationship of the fi ngers to one

another interferes with manipulation skills.

Spinocerebellar Pathways

 Crucial as a foundation for movement and tone,

proprioceptive inputs are also projected to the

cerebellum ( Fig. 4-16 ). The cerebellum uses this

input, along with vestibular input, to monitor

body and limb position in and movement through

space, and to regulate the timing of movement.

Spinocerebellar inputs form the dorsal spinocerebellar pathway, carrying proprioceptive input

from the lower part of the body, and the cuneocerebellar pathway, carrying proprioceptive input

from the upper extremities. In addition, proprioceptive information from the face is relayed

to the cerebellum from the trigeminal mesencephalic nucleus.

78 ■ PART II The Neuroscience Basis of Sensory Integration Disorders

Anterolateral (AL) System

 The AL ( Fig. 4-17 ) is composed of separate pathways that function primarily to transmit pain,

crude touch (the detection of an object ’ s position

but not its movement across the skin), and temperature. Neutral warmth and the “tickle” sensation are also related to transmission within these

anterolateral pathways. The term anterolateral

system is sometimes used interchangeably with

spinothalamic pathway because the thalamus is

a major projection point for many fi bers traveling

in the anterolateral fasciculus. In fact, some texts

have indicated that the AL is but one pathway,

the spinothalamic pathway, with intermediate

projections to the reticular formation, cranial

nerve nuclei, and parts of the mesencephalon

and hypothalamus on the way to the thalamus.

Individual projections are sometimes classifi ed

as specifi c pathways (spinothalamic, spinoreticular, spinobulbar, spinomesencephalic, and spinohypothalamic) but sometimes not. Determining

which nomenclature is correct is beyond the

FIGURE 4-16 Spinocerebellar pathways (dorsal, ventral, and cuneo) carrying proprioceptive information from

the body to the cerebellum.

Parietal lobe

Somatosensory

cortex

Thigh

area

Leg area

Foot area

Perineal area

Trunk

Arm area

area

Forearm

area

Hand

area

Face

area

VPL

Superior cerebellar peduncle

Cuneocerebellar tract

Inferior cerebellar

peduncle

Nucleus

gracilis

(NG)

Nucleus

cuneatus

(NC)

Accessory

cuneate nucleus

(ACN)

Medial lemniscus

NG

NC

ACN

Ventral spinocerebellar tract

Dorsal spinocerebellar tract

Ventral spinocerebellar tract

Dorsal spinocerebellar tract

Muscle spindles, cutaneous and

joint receptors

Muscle spindles

Muscle spindles, cutaneous and

joint receptors

Muscle spindles

Alpha motor neuron

Ventral spinocerebellar tract

Dorsal spinocerebellar tract

Alpha motor neuron

Fasciculus cuneatus

Fasciculus gracilis

Fasciculus gracilis

Fasciculus dorsalis

Cutaneous, joint

and muscle receptors

Spinal cord L1

Spinal cord T1

Lower medulla

CHAPTER 4 Structure and Function of the Sensory Systems ■ 79

scope of this text. For clarity in identifying the

beginning and end of the projections, we will use

the individual pathway designations.

 Receptors for the AL system include those that

respond to rough stimuli (e.g., rubbing, squeezing, pinching) that do not result in tissue damage

as well as those that do respond when tissue is

damaged. These latter receptors are mechanonocioceptors. When tissue is damaged, the release

of chemical substances activates a third class of

receptors, called chemonocioceptors. There are

also receptors for sensations of cold and heat.

None of these receptors localizes inputs well

when compared with receptors associated with

the DCML.

 As with the DCML, cell bodies for neurons

associated with the AL are in the dorsal root ganglion. Projections from dorsal root ganglion cells

enter the spinal cord, and the fi bers ascend or

descend one or two spinal segments before synapsing in the dorsal horn. The interconnections

of these fi bers can be complex. After synapsing, the majority of second-order neurons cross

to the other side of the cord and project to the

brainstem reticular formation and the thalamus.

The crossing pattern defi nes a different picture

for injury or dysfunction than that seen for the

DCML. Any injury to this system above the level

of fi ber entry into the spinal cord results in defi -

cits on the opposite side of the body.

 As suggested by the pathway names, information carried within the AL system projects to

the reticular system (spinoreticular), thalamus

(spinothalamic), periaqueductal gray and the

tectum (spinomesencephalic), and hypothalamic

(spinohypothalamic) areas. Interestingly, a large

portion of fi bers within the AL terminate within

the reticular formation. The transmission of both

diffuse and chronic pain is thought to be projected to this area of the brain, where arousal is

associated with pain. Spinothalamic projections

carrying nonspecifi c touch, temperature, and pain

FIGURE 4-17 The anterolateral pathway consists of several fi ber pathways. Shown here are the spinoreticular,

spinomesencephalic, and spinothalamic.

Lumbar

spinal

cord

Cervical

spinal

 cord

Cerebrum

Midbrain

Pons

Medulla

Thalamus and

hypothalamus

Periaqueductal

gray matter

Reticular formation

Reticular formation

Locus coeruleus

Somatosensory cortex

and limbic system

Spinothalamic tract

Spinoreticular tract

Spinomesencephalic tract

80 ■ PART II The Neuroscience Basis of Sensory Integration Disorders

go to the VPL as well as other thalamic nuclei.

The thalamus also receives tactile projections

from the reticular formation. Fibers sent to midbrain regions (periaqueductal gray and tectum)

and the hypothalamus permit information about

pain to become available to the limbic system

and the ANS, generating emotional, neuroendocrine, and cardiovascular responses to pain.

Interestingly, the emotional components of pain

can be separated from pain perception, probably because of the variety of projections within

the AL. Medications in the benzodiazepine class

(e.g., Valium), when used for pain, do not mask

pain perception. Instead, they make the sensation

of pain less distressing through their action on

the limbic system. Projections to the tectum may

be associated with the visual (superior colliculus) and auditory (inferior colliculus) systems;

however, the tectum is also an important center

for pain reception.

 Perception of pain relies on projections to

the VPL of the thalamus, where it may be interpreted as paresthesia or dull pain and pressure.

Pain projections in the thalamus are more widespread than those from the DCML, and the projections for pain and touch remain segregated

( Bear et al., 2015 ). Projections from the VPL

go to the somatosensory cortices (both S-I and

S-II), which, therefore, are also potential anatomic sites for interaction between DCML and

AL inputs. Precise localization of pain is thought

to take place at the cortical level.

Somatosensation from the Face

 The trigeminothalamic pathway ( Fig. 4-18 )

transmits tactile and proprioceptive input from

the face. The cell bodies for the fi bers in the

peripheral aspect of this pathway are located

in the trigeminal ganglion. From there, fi bers

project to the pons and the spinal cord, where

they both ascend and descend before synapsing.

Fibers carrying pain and temperature information

form the spinal trigeminal tract. From there projections go to the contralateral ventral posterior

medial thalamic nucleus and onto the primary

and secondary sensory cortices; these projections

provide the discriminative aspect of facial pain.

The affective aspects of pain follow pathways

to the reticular formation, areas in the midbrain,

and midline thalamic nuclei, which then connect

with the cingulate gyrus. Pain and temperature

information from the face are also transmitted by

way of the facial, glossopharyngeal, and vagus

cranial nerves, VII, IX, and X, respectively.

 Cell bodies for neurons carrying proprioceptive information from muscles of the face are

found in the mesencephalic trigeminal nucleus,

located in the midbrain. From here, this information projects to the thalamus and on to the primary

sensory cortex, where the regions around the

mouth have a wide representation. The sensory

homunculus refl ects this wide representation.

Functional Considerations

 In SI theory, the tactile system is thought to be

of the utmost importance in determining behavior. The sensation of touch is, in fact, the “oldest

and most primitive expressive channel” ( Collier,

 1985 , p. 29), and it is a primary system for

making contact with the external world. We are

extremely dependent on touch until language,

motor skills, and cognitive processes develop

and can guide our experiences and interactions

( Collier, 1985 ; Diamond & Hopson, 1998 ). Touch

has been called our fi rst language; it is the fi rst

system to function in utero, and it mediates our

fi rst experiences in this world. We are nourished,

we are calmed, and we fi rst become attached to

others (i.e., bonding) through touch ( Montagu,

 1978 ). The somatosensory system differs from

other sensory systems in that the receptors are

widely distributed ( Bear et al., 2015 ), a fact that

has potential ramifi cations from a sensory integrative perspective; we can close our eyes when

light is too bright or put fi ngers in our ears to

block out sound, but it is very diffi cult to effectively “turn-down” tactile input. This system also

differs in that it responds to multiple types of

sensation rather than the more unitary responses

seen with other sensory systems.

 In a review of literature, Blackwell ( 2000 )

summarized the power of the tactile sensory

system:

 There remains little doubt that tactile stimulation

is an important factor in the social, emotional,

physiological, and neurological development of

infants and young children. Consequently, it is

one of the most essential elements in the nurturing and healing environment of the infant and

child. (p. 37)

 With widespread receptors that respond to

several different types of input, the potential

CHAPTER 4 Structure and Function of the Sensory Systems ■ 81

for multiple occupational roles to be disrupted

by inadequacies in processing tactile input is

worth considering. For example, a diffi cult time

with performance of activities of daily living

(ADLs) may be related to inadequate integration

of input from tactile receptors responsible for

discrimination. Poor student performance may

result from diffi culty manipulating writing and

cutting tools in the classroom. Poor peer interactions may result from inadequate modulation of

tactile sensation. Many aspects of touch associated with tactile defensiveness are hypothetically

associated with transmission through the AL

pathways and with the central interpretation of

the input ( Ayres, 1972 ). Given that the AL pathways project to the regions of the brain responsible for arousal (reticular system), emotional

tone (limbic structures), and autonomic regulation (hypothalamus), we postulate that tactiledefensive behaviors may be related to the

connections among these systems and brain

regions. Tactile over-responsivity is discussed in

more detail in Chapter 6 (Sensory Modulation

Functions and Disorders).

 Although functionally it appears that the

DCML and the AL are separate and discrete,

considerable functional overlap can be seen. For

example, the DCML plays an important role in

the localization of pain. Furthermore, children

with lesions in the DCML retain some skill in

tactile discrimination. Thus, some information

about pain is transmitted through the DCML,

and some aspects of tactile discrimination must

be carried in the AL. Many authors have discussed this redundancy of function in terms of

parallel pathways and serial processing. Parallel

pathways are advantageous because they add to

the depth and fl avor of a perceptual experience

by allowing the same information to be handled

in different ways, and they offer a measure of

FIGURE 4-18 Trigeminothalamic pathway. This pathway carries light touch, pain, temperature, deep touch, and

proprioception from the face to the thalamus.

Cerebral

cortex and

thalamus

Midbrain

Dorsal trigeminothalamic tract

Pons

Mechanoreceptive

fibers in cranial

nerves V, VII, IX, and X

Posterior

limb of

internal

capsule

Primary

somatic

sensory

cortex

Secondary

somatic

sensory

cortex

Ventral

posterior

medial

nucleus

Trigeminal lemniscus

Main trigeminal

sensory nucleus

82 ■ PART II The Neuroscience Basis of Sensory Integration Disorders

insurance. If one pathway is damaged, the other

can provide residual perceptual capability. Such

functional redundancy in the organization of the

nervous system may play a role in the effi cacy of

intervention.

 Proprioceptive inputs to the cortex combine

with tactile inputs to support the somatotopic

mapping associated with this region. In fact, in

the sensory cortex there are four complete maps

of the body (homunculus), one each in area 3a,

3b, 1, and 2; recent research has identifi ed

area 3b as the primary sensory cortex. Maps

in each of these regions refl ect the density of

sensory receptors on the body. Because there

is greater density of receptors in regions where

more precise information is required for function,

these regions appear large in the homunculus.

The area associated with the hand, for instance,

is quite large, and the thumb/index region even

larger. Similarly, the area associated with the

mouth is disproportionately large, refl ecting

the important role that somatosensation plays

in speech and eating. As noted earlier, area 3a

receives substantial proprioceptive input, giving

it a primary role in the sense of body position.

 Proprioceptive inputs to the cerebellum contribute to our ability to monitor and adjust movement as it takes place. The cerebellum has an

error correction function, resulting in our ability

to make changes in motor output to meet changing environmental demand. Proprioceptive inputs

then support the comparator function of the cerebellum, allowing the cerebellum to compare the

plan for action with the action itself as it unfolds.

The end result of this process is the production

of smooth, coordinated, multi-joint action. When

there is damage to the spinocerebellum, disrupting the processing of proprioceptive input

and feedback, it may be seen as a wide, shuffl ing gait, diffi culty performing rapid alternating

movements, and over- and under-reaching. Interestingly, cerebellar inputs remain ipsilateral; this

means that when there are cerebellar defi cits,

they are seen on the same side of the body. This

is in contrast to the processing of proprioception

at higher levels, which is contralateral.

HERE ’ S THE POINT

 • Receptors for touch are found primarily in the

layers of the skin; those for proprioception are

found in muscles, joints, and tendons.

 • Somatosensory information from the body

travels from receptors to the cerebellum

(spinocerebellar pathways for proprioception),

regions of the reticular formation,

hypothalamus, limbic system, and thalamus

(anterolateral system and dorsal column medial

lemniscal pathway), and from the thalamus to

the primary and secondary sensory cortices, as

well as areas 5 and 7 of the parietal lobe.

 • Somatosensory information from the face is

carried by the trigeminothalamic pathway

to the thalamus and from there projects to

regions of the cortex for the somatosensation

from the body.

 • Somatosensory receptors are pervasive

throughout the head and body; their central

influences are broad. These characteristics

support widespread infl uence of this sensory

input on occupational engagement and

participation.

Interoception

 Although the perception of sensation relative

to the internal body has not been a focus of SI,

it warrants consideration here. Interoception

involves sensing the physiological condition of

the body. Thus, we are aware of such internal

sensations as hunger, satiety and thirst, heartbeat,

and visceral sensations. As will be discussed

next, the interoceptive pathways function in conjunction with motor and autonomic pathways,

giving us the ability to have an internal sense of

self and a means of acting on the environment.

Receptors and Transduction

 We have already addressed one set of receptors for this system; they include the tactile

receptors described previously for the anterolateral pathway. This may seem counterintuitive; although Sherrington ( 1906 ) fi rst coined

the term interoception to include only information from the interior of the body (viscera),

more recently Craig ( 2002, 2009 ) indicated

that we needed to broaden our defi nition of this

term to include all sensory inputs that provide

the CNS with information regarding how the

body, internal and external, feels. Receptors in

our internal organs and blood vessels, schematically shown in Figure 4-4 , include free nerve

CHAPTER 4 Structure and Function of the Sensory Systems ■ 83

endings, mechanoreceptors, chemoreceptors, and

thermoreceptors. Cell bodies of primary afferent neurons reside in spinal and cranial nerve

ganglia. Interoceptive input travels to the CNS

over small diameter fi bers, and it provides us

with an understanding of our internal state.

 One rationale for including skin receptors as

interoceptors has to do with their role in providing the CNS with information about the state

of the body; another rationale has to do with

how the receptors are activated and how this

information is transmitted. In contrast to other

somatosensory input, sensations of pain, itch, or

temperature can be activated by internal stimuli

as well as external or mechanical stimuli. And

they are activated continuously, providing us

with ongoing information about our physiological state. Interoceptive signals, such as pain,

temperature, and itch sensation, as well as other

chemical and hormonal signals, are projected to

the spinal cord over relatively small fi bers. As

such, interoceptors are the basis for homeostasis.

Interpreting Interoceptive Input

 The projection target for interoceptive fi bers

entering the cord is in one particular region,

lamina 1 ( Fig. 4-19 ). Within lamina 1 are

modality-specifi c cells that respond to changes

in blood chemistry (e.g., oxygenation, glucose

levels), hormone levels, and by-products from

muscle activation. Fibers projecting from lamina

1 go to autonomic cell columns and homeostatic

centers in the brainstem. In the brainstem, tactile

information from the body is joined by sensory

information from the heart, viscera, tongue, and

pharynx, conducting information related to such

things as taste, thirst, nausea, and dyspnea (diffi -

culty breathing), projecting to the solitary nucleus

in the medulla. The combination of these two

sources of interoceptive input forms what is considered the afferent or input aspect of the ANS,

allowing us to map an internal awareness of self

and integrate sensation related to functions of

the ANS and homeostasis. Multiple connections

take place in the brainstem, and from here these

integrated sensations are sent to the hypothalamus, amygdala, and thalamus. The hypothalamus

is the seat of autonomic regulation and homeostasis. Activation of the cells in this structure

leads to hormonal, visceral, and somatic motor

responses designed to re-establish the body ’ s

status quo. Hypothalamic hormones are responsible for negotiating signals refl ecting changes in

temperature, thirst, hunger, sleep, stress, mood,

and sex drive. The amygdala is linked with fear

conditioning, regulating our autonomic and emotional responses to environmental danger. Projections of interoception then continue to the insular

cortex, where research has identifi ed sensory

maps of the body, and groups of cells with some

differential responsiveness, that defi ne distinct

sensations (such as sharp pain) and less distinct

interoceptive sensations (such as gastric distention) ( Craig, 2015 ). This region of the cortex is

linked with actual and perceived stimulus intensity and, consistent with its role in interoception,

tracking the body ’ s physiological state ( Uddin

& Menon, 2009 ). The insula is also important

in attaching emotional signifi cance to body state

(for instance, a rested body state may be associated with feeling happy), and in our awareness

of environment, self, and others ( Craig, 2009 ).

These sensations are linked with homeostasis,

and they guide emotional behaviors. There are

also interoceptive projections to the anterior cingulate cortex, termed the limbic motor cortex,

guiding action in homeostasis.

Functional Considerations

 Recent research suggests defi cits in interoceptive

processing in individuals with autism spectrum

disorder (ASD) ( Elwin, Schröder, Ek, & Kjellin,

 2012 ; Fiene & Brownlow, 2015 ). Participants

in the study by Elwin and colleagues indicated

they experienced over-responsivity to external

sensation and under-responsivity to inner/body

sensation. The work of Fiene and Brownlow

FIGURE 4-19 A cross section of the spinal cord

showing spinal lamina (left) and associated labels for

cell groups (right).

I

Central canal

Marginal zone

Gelatinous

substance

Nucleus

proprius

Lateral motor

neurons

Medial motor

neurons

II

III

IV

V

VI

VII

VIII

IX

X

84 ■ PART II The Neuroscience Basis of Sensory Integration Disorders

extended these fi ndings, looking more specifi -

cally at internal body awareness in individuals

with ASD. These investigators found reduced

awareness of the body and feelings of thirst in

adults with autism. The anterior insula, a region

thought to process interoceptive input, has been

shown repeatedly to be under-active in individuals with ASD (see Di Martino et al., 2009

for meta-analysis). Regions of the insula, along

with the anterior cingulate cortex, are also linked

with empathy. Individuals with autism, as well

as typical individuals with lower empathy, show

decreased activity in the anterior insula, leading

Uddin and Menon to suggest that these limbicsystem-linked structures function in social and

emotional responses relative to both the self and

others. Given the importance of interoception for

homeostatic regulation and its apparent contributions to social functioning, a better understanding

of interoceptive processing and its relationship

to processing within other sensory systems and

functional behaviors is warranted.

HERE ’ S THE POINT

 • Interoception, the sense we have of the

physiological condition of the body, is detected

by receptors in our internal organs and

subsequently projected to the spinal cord and

onto the brainstem, forming the afferent arm

of the ANS. Some tactile inputs are linked to

interoception.

 • From the brainstem, interoceptive information

is sent to the hypothalamus, thalamus, and

amygdala and then on to the insular cortex.

These connections subserve our knowledge of

our body ’ s physiological state, link this with

emotion, and provide us with an awareness of

the environment.

 • Under-responsiveness to interoceptive signals

has been identifi ed in individuals with ASD.

The Vestibular System

 We approach the vestibular system in the same

manner as the somatosensory system, beginning with receptor structure and function and

then examining vestibular projections within the

CNS. Similar to other sensory systems, there are

both peripheral and central components to the

vestibular system, comprised of the receptors

and vestibular nerve fi bers (peripheral) and the

vestibular nuclei and multiple projection pathways (central). Following the vestibular system,

we will discuss proprioception from a functional

perspective and briefl y look at the interaction

between vestibular and proprioceptive sensation as they relate to the control of posture and

movement.

Receptors and Transduction

 The vestibular apparatus sits inside the body labyrinth within the temporal bone, adjacent to the

cochlea ( Fig. 4-20 ). It includes the semicircular

canals and the otolith organs, the utricle and the

saccule ( Fig. 4-21 ). Receptors for the vestibular system are located within these structures in

the inner ear, and the endolymph that bathes the

receptors for the auditory system moves freely

between the auditory and vestibular systems.

 Vestibular receptors are hair cells located in

the otolith organs and in swellings at the base

of the three semicircular canals (i.e., anterior,

lateral, posterior). The otolith organs are responsible primarily for static functions, concerned

with head translation and changes in head position relative to gravity. The information processed by these receptors is used to detect the

position of the head and body in space as well

as control of posture. The semicircular canals are

the dynamic component of the vestibular system,

responding to rotation and angular movement of

the head. These structures respond to movement

of the head in space.

 Vestibular receptors are chalice-shaped with

hair-like processes extending from their apices.

At the base of each cell lies the afferent process

of the vestibular nerve fi bers. The vestibular

component of the vestibular-auditory cranial

nerve is formed from the axons of these fi bers.

Each cell has a single kinocilium and several

stereocilia. Although movement of the kinocilium in one direction leads to depolarization of

the hair cell, movement in the opposite direction

leads to hyperpolarization. Hair cells within each

semicircular canal have a specifi c orientation

such that they are all depolarized or hyperpolarized by movement in one direction. Cells in the

otolith organs are also polarized, but, because of

the structure of these organs, cells respond to a

vast array of directions of movement. When the

otolith cells depolarize, glutamate, an excitatory

CHAPTER 4 Structure and Function of the Sensory Systems ■ 85

neurotransmitter, is released into the synaptic

cleft. The transmitter interacts with the afferent

fi ber of the vestibular nerve, sending information about movement to the CNS ( Soto & Vega,

 2010 ). We discuss the specifi cs of depolarization

within the semicircular canals and otolith organs

next.

 In addition to the afferent fi bers at the base of

each hair cell, there are also efferent fi bers that

originate in the vestibular nuclei. These efferent

fi bers provide inhibitory control of the transmission of information from the hair cell and can

prevent information from traveling beyond the

receptor site.

Utricle and Saccule

 The otoliths are saclike organs oriented in horizontal and vertical planes ( Fig. 4-21 ). In the

macula, which is the receptor region of the

otolith organs, hair cells synapse with processes

from the vestibular ganglion cells. Hair cell processes extend into an overlying substance with

gelatin-like qualities, the otolith membrane, in

which are embedded otoconia (calcium carbonate crystals). In the upright position, the hair

cells in the macula of the utricle are oriented in

a horizontal plane, whereas those of the saccule

are oriented in the vertical plane. In both sets of

organs, the otoconia rest on top of the hair cells.

When the head tilts or moves in any plane (side

to side, up/down, forward/backward), there is displacement of the otoconia and the embedded hair

cell stereocilia, beginning the process of stimulus detection and transduction. Movement of the

stereocilia creates electrical discharges within

the hair cell. This electrical energy is changed

to chemical energy at the synapse between the

macular hair cells and the vestibular ganglion

projections. Thus, together, the utricle and saccule

respond to head tilt in any direction and to linear

movement. It is important to remember that we

get directionality because the system is bilateral

and these structures are paired; activation on one

side of the head is matched with inhibition on the

FIGURE 4-20 Vestibular and auditory receptor systems are both located in the temporal bone, in close proximity

to each other.

(1) Auricle

Temporal bone

(4) Malleus

(5) Incus

(6) Stapes

(10) Semicircular canals

Vestibular

branch

Cochlear

branch

(7) Cochlea

Vestibulocochlear

nerve

(8) Oval window (3) Tympanic

 membrane

(11) Vestibule

(2) External auditory

canal

External ear Middle ear Inner ear

(9) Eustachian tube

86 ■ PART II The Neuroscience Basis of Sensory Integration Disorders

other side of the head. The otolith maculae constitute slow-adapting receptors and provide tonic

input to the CNS pertaining to head position and

movement. The tonic input is crucial in supporting upright posture and equilibrium.

Semicircular Canals

 The semicircular canals ( Fig. 4-21 ), which are

actually closed tubes, detect changes in the direction and rate of angular acceleration or deceleration of the head. Angular acceleration results

FIGURE 4-21 Structures of the inner ear. The vestibular apparatus on the left side of the image shows three

semicircular canals as well as the receptor region in the ampulla at the swelling of one of the canals. Within

the ampulla is the crista, which consists of the hair cells and the overlying gelatinous mass, called the cupula.

The otolith organs, the utricle and the saccule, are also identifi ed. Within the saccule, the macula, or receptor

region, is shown.

Semicircular canals

Endolymph

Crista

Saccule

Vestibular nerve

Cochlear nerve

Vestibulocochlear

nerve

Scala

tympani

Cochlear

duct

Scala

vestibuli

Cochlea

Round window

Oval window

Utricle

Ampule

Kinocilium

Type I hair cell

Nerve fiber

Basement membrane

Supporting cells

Type II hair cell

Stereocilia

Otolithic

membrane

Otoconia

CHAPTER 4 Structure and Function of the Sensory Systems ■ 87

in rotary head movements—that is, head movements that, if continued far enough, would result

in the head turning in a circle (e.g., spinning,

head nodding). Within each vestibular apparatus, the three semicircular canals are oriented

at right angles so that they represent all three

planes in space. When the head is tilted forward

30 degrees, the horizontal canal is oriented in the

horizontal plane, with the anterior and posterior

canals positioned vertically and oriented at right

angles to each other.

 The semicircular canals have an enlarged

ending called the ampulla. Within the ampulla

is the receptor apparatus for the semicircular

canals, the crista ampullaris, which contains the

hair cell receptors. The receptors are embedded in the cupula, a substance with gelatin-like

characteristics similar to the macula. There are

no otoconia in the crista ampullaris. Instead, the

cupula extends nearly to the top of the ampulla,

and its edges are, for the most part, anchored to

the epithelium that lines the canal. The canals

are fi lled with endolymph. When the head moves

(accelerates), inertia causes the endolymph to lag

behind head movement. This is often paralleled

with swirling water in a glass; when you begin

to swirl the glass, you can see that water “lags”

behind the speed of movement of the glass. If

you continue to swirl, the water speed will catch

up, and the glass and water will move “as one.”

In the semicircular canals, the initial lag results

in pressure on the cupula and its displacement in

a direction opposite to that of head movement.

Displacement of the cupula leads to bending of

the hairs, and this mechanical distortion begins

the process of transduction. As head movement

continues, the speed of the endolymph catches

up with that of the head, the cupula returns to its

resting position, and the hair cells are no longer

mechanically distorted. With continued movement of the head at a relatively constant velocity,

the semicircular canal receptors return to a basal

fi ring rate. When head movement stops or decelerates, the inertia again acts on the endolymph,

and it continues to move in the canals, this time

in the direction of head movement. Again, pressure is placed on the cupula, bending the hairs in

the same direction in which the head had been

moving. This then changes transmission and

activity in the vestibular nerve. Several seconds

after the head stops moving, the cupula and hairs

return to their normal resting positions.

 As is the case for the otolith organs, the

semicircular canals are paired structures. The

horizontal canal on one side is paired with

the horizontal canal on the other side of the head,

whereas one anterior canal is paired with the

posterior canal on the opposite side of the head.

The alignment of hair cells in the crista ampullaris of each member of the pair is opposite, so

when the hair cells on one side are excited, those

in the matching canal on the other side are inhibited. As was the case with the otolith organs, this

orientation and pairing provides directionality to

movement.

 Afferent and efferent fi bers meet the hair cells

at their base. Afferent fi bers carry information

from the receptors to the vestibular ganglion,

and, from there, fi bers join to become the vestibular nerve ( Fig. 4-21 ), one part of cranial nerve

VIII, the vestibulocochlear nerve. The vestibular fi bers project to the vestibular nuclei. Efferent input from the nuclei may form part of an

early feedback mechanism within the vestibular

system or may serve to guide typical development of vestibular structures ( Baloh & Kerber,

 2010 ). Investigation continues to be warranted

on the precise function of these efferent fi bers;

however, output from these fi bers results in

modifi cation and, potentially, modulation ( Holt,

Lysakowski, & Goldberg, 2011 ).

 Because the hair cells in each pair of canals

are maximally stimulated by head rotation in the

same plane, they are able to detect movement

of the head in the three orthogonal (right-angle)

planes of three-dimensional space. The most

effi cient stimuli for the semicircular canals are

angular, transient (short-term), and fast (highfrequency) head movements of at least 2 degrees

per second. When the head moves at slower

speeds, the endolymph, cupula, and hair cells all

move at the same speed as the head. As such,

the cupula does not bend, and hair cells in the

ampulla are not activated.

 As noted earlier, the otolith inputs are a

foundation for upright posture and equilibrium

because these inputs are tonic. Semicircular

canal inputs are phasic; sensory information is

sent only as long as the cupula bends the hair

cells. These inputs then are important in triggering righting responses, supporting our ability

to master perturbations from movement in and

through the environment. Afferent fi bers in the

vestibular nerve transmit both tonic and phasic

88 ■ PART II The Neuroscience Basis of Sensory Integration Disorders

information from the receptors, which is critical

to function within this system.

Central Projections

 There is always some activity within the vestibular nerve, primarily because of tonic activation

of otolith organs by gravity. Activation of the

receptor organs either increases or decreases

this baseline activity, depending on the type and

direction of movement. Activation in one half of

the pair of canals is met with decreased activity

in the parallel canal on the other side of the head.

Because the vestibular nerves project both ipsilaterally and contralaterally, the vestibular nuclei

interpret direction of movement by comparing

the frequency of impulse fl ow between left and

right canals and otolith organs.

 The cell bodies for the vestibular nerve are

located in the vestibular nerve ganglion, also

known as Scarpa’s ganglion. From these cell

bodies, the vestibular nerve carries information

to the vestibular nuclei in the brainstem. There

are four nuclei on each side: lateral, medial,

superior, and inferior ( Fig. 4-22 panel A). A great

deal of sensory processing takes place at this

level within the vestibular system. Each nucleus

receives direct ipsilateral input as well as contralateral input via fi bers crossing from the opposite

nuclei. These nuclei also receive inputs from the

spinal cord, cerebellum, and visual system. The

organization of these inputs allows detection of

direction and speed of head movement as well as

position of the head relative to gravity.

 Vestibular nuclei receive input from other

sensory systems, notably the visual system.

According to Purves and colleagues ( 2011 ),

this makes the vestibular system unique among

sensory systems; there is multisensory integration in the fi rst site of central vestibular processing and throughout all subsequent projections.

Visual inputs are relayed through the inferior

olive and cerebellum, and the interaction of these

inputs is thought to be important in generating

eye movements, as described later.

 From the vestibular nuclei come many fi ber

pathways that connect the vestibular system

extensively within the CNS. Direct connections are found between vestibular nuclei and

the cerebellum, oculomotor nuclei, and spinal

cord. Projections have also been described to

parts of the reticular system, the thalamus, and

the cortex (frontal lobe, parietal-insular cortex).

This organization within the vestibular system

is an example of heterarchical processing rather

than hierarchical; each connection has a unique

function.

Vestibular-Cerebellar Connections

 The vestibular system is the only sensory system

with direct connections from receptors to the

cerebellum. Projections come from the vestibular nerve directly ( Fig. 4-22 ) and from vestibular

nuclei. In turn, there are direct connections from

the cerebellum to the vestibular nuclei. These

reciprocal connections from the cerebellum go

primarily to the medial and lateral vestibular

nuclei. As noted later, ascending fi bers from the

medial vestibular nucleus project to oculomotor

nuclei, allowing the cerebellum to infl uence eye

position; descending projections from this same

nucleus give the cerebellum control of head and

neck movements ( Fig. 4-22 panel A). Reciprocal connections between the cerebellum and the

lateral vestibular nucleus infl uence output over

the lateral vestibulospinal pathway, thus infl uencing postural control. Together then, the vestibular

system and the cerebellum coordinate eye, head,

and trunk movements and are critical for posture,

balance, and equilibrium.

Vestibulospinal Connections

 The vestibular nuclei send projections to the

spinal cord via lateral and medial vestibulospinal pathways (LVST and MVST, respectively;

 Fig. 4-22 panel A). These pathways are responsible for infl uences on muscle tone as well as

for ongoing postural adjustments. The LVST

receives input from semicircular canal pairs,

otolith organs, the vestibulocerebellum, and the

spinal cord. Fibers from the LVST terminate

directly on alpha and gamma motor neurons in

the spinal cord at the cervical, lumbar, and sacral

levels. Alpha motor neurons supply muscle

fi bers, and gamma motor neurons project to the

muscle spindle; thus, the vestibular system has

a strong infl uence on postural muscles, postural

control, and stability. The MVST receives input

from the cerebellum and from skin and joint proprioceptors. The fi bers in this pathway project

to fl exor and extensor motor neurons in the cervical region of the cord. This input assists with

the maintenance of a consistent position of the

head in space. Thus, with descending vestibular

CHAPTER 4 Structure and Function of the Sensory Systems ■ 89

projections, we see the interaction of vestibular

and proprioceptive inputs.

 Responses elicited because of stimulation of

the utricle or semicircular canal activate extensor muscles, eliciting compensatory movements

of the head, trunk, and limbs. Such movements

help oppose head perturbations, postural sway,

or tilt and keep us upright ( Fisher & Bundy,

 1989 ; Goldberg et al., 2012 ). However, as might

be expected, there are differences between the

kinds of postural responses ultimately elicited by

stimulation to the different receptors. Utricular

inputs, conveyed primarily via the LVST to limb

and upper trunk alpha and gamma motoneurons, result in ipsilateral facilitation of extensor

muscles and inhibition of fl exor muscles. Semicircular canal inputs are conveyed primarily via

the medial vestibulospinal pathway to axial alpha

and gamma motoneurons and result in bilateral facilitation of neck and upper-trunk fl exor

muscles. Functionally utricular inputs elicit more

sustained postural responses (i.e., tonic postural

extension and support reactions); semicircular canal inputs elicit more phasic equilibrium

FIGURE 4-22 A. Central vestibular connections. Ascending fi bers connect with oculomotor nuclei to coordinate

movement of the eyes relative to the head. Vestibular projections are also found to the thalamus and on to

regions in the cortex. The vestibular system has a reciprocal connection with the cerebellum, and fi bers descend

to the spinal cord as the medial and lateral vestibulospinal pathways. B. One of the cortical projection regions of

the vestibular system, found at the base of the precentral gyrus and the intraparietal sulcus.

Vestibular nuclei

Medial vestibulospinal tract

Lateral vestibulospinal tract

Anterior

semicircular canal

Posterior

semicircular canal

Horizontal

semicircular canal

Utricle

A Saccule

B

Inferior

cerebellar

peduncle

Fastigial

nucleus

Vestibulocerebellum

Feedback loop

Ventral posterior nucleus

in thalamus

Vestibular area in

cerebral cortex

Oculomotor nerve

(III) nucleus

Trochlear nerve (IV)

motor nucleus

Abduceus nerve (VI)

motor nucleus

Vestibular

cortex

90 ■ PART II The Neuroscience Basis of Sensory Integration Disorders

responses ( Fisher & Bundy, 1989 ; Roberts, 1978 ;

 Wilson & Melvill Jones, 1979 ).

 Functionally then, if the goal is to facilitate

tonic postural or support reactions, activities that

provide utricular stimulation may be more appropriate. If the goal is to encourage the use of more

phasic or transient postural reactions, then activities that provide stimulation to the semicircular

canal may be indicated.

Vestibular-Oculomotor Connections

 Vestibular fi bers directly project to oculomotor

nuclei for cranial nerves III (oculomotor), IV

(trochlear), and VI (abducens) via the medial

longitudinal fasciculus ( Fig. 4-22 panel A).

Fibers are both crossed and uncrossed as they

reach these nuclei. These connections serve to

provide an ongoing stable visual image; as the

head turns in one direction, eye movement occurs

in the opposite direction, preventing retinal slip

and maintaining visual acuity ( Goldberg et al.,

 2012 ). Inputs that mediate these responses come

from the semicircular canals and are active in

any plane of movement. When the head is not

moving, the eyes remain still. However, with head

movement comes activation of the vestibuloocular refl ex to enable the visual fi eld to remain

stable even as the head and body move.

Nystagmus is a specialized compensatory

vestibulo-ocular movement. As the head moves

in an angular fashion, interactions between the

oculomotor nuclei and the vestibular system

allow the eyes to remain fi xed on an object in

space. With continued angular movement of the

head, the eyes reach the end of their range of

motion; this comprises the slow phase of nystagmus. Once the end of range is reached, the

eyes spring back to a central position; the quick

movement to a central position comprises the

fast phase of nystagmus. The nystagmic eye

movements are tied to the movement of endolymph in the semicircular canals, which results

from angular movement of the head. At the

onset of head movement, endolymph movement

lags behind head movement, bending the cupula

in the semicircular canal as it moves. As head

movement continues at a steady pace, the speed

of endolymph movement catches up with the

speed of head movement, and the cupula regains

an upright position. This stops activation of the

hair cell receptors, and input to the CNS returns

to baseline.

 Nystagmus is named for the direction of the

fast phase, which is the same as the direction of

head movement. When nystagmus occurs during

head movement, it is termed per-rotary nystagmus (i.e., nystagmus that takes place during

movement). Per-rotary nystagmus declines and

eventually stops as described previously. When

the head stops, endolymph in the canals continues to move in the direction of head turn. This

activates the cupula again, in the opposite direction, and triggers the same sequence of events

described previously but in the opposite direction, this time leading to postrotary nystagmus.

 Measurement of postrotary nystagmus is a

tool that has been used to examine one aspect

of the integrity of the vestibular system. When

using this measurement, it is important to have

a more complete understanding of the processes

underlying nystagmus. These processes were

well described by Fisher ( 1989 ); we summarize

them briefl y here. Movement of endolymph in

the semicircular canal and displacement of the

cupula initiate nystagmus. However, the cupula

returns to a resting position and stops activating

the vestibular receptors several seconds before

nystagmus ceases. This phenomenon is related

to velocity storage, a mechanism associated

with the vestibular nuclei in which velocity

information generated by movement is collected

and stored and then released slowly, generating

nystagmus ( Baloh & Kerber, 2010 ; Goldberg

et al., 2012 ). Fisher, Mixon, and Herman ( 1986 )

suggested that this mechanism was impaired in

individuals with a vestibular-based dysfunction

in SI, resulting in shortened duration of postrotary nystagmus. Further investigation is needed

to confi rm this possibility.

 Vestibular-Thalamic

and Cortical Connections

 Vestibular connections project bilaterally to the

VPL of the thalamus as well as to the lateral and

paramedian nuclear groups of the thalamus. The

VPL receives somatosensory input and is one

anatomic region where interaction of somatosensory and vestibular inputs takes place. From the

thalamus, fi bers project to the cortex, to the base

of the precentral gyrus (area 3a), and to the base

of intraparietal sulcus (area 2V) ( Goldberg et al.,

 2012 ) ( Fig. 4-22 panel B). Area 2V neurons

respond to head movements, and activation of

this region leads to sensations of dizziness or

CHAPTER 4 Structure and Function of the Sensory Systems ■ 91

awareness of movement. Neurons here receive

not only vestibular input but also visual and proprioceptive inputs, and this area is likely involved

with the perception of motion and spatial orientation. A lesion here leads to confusion in spatial

orientation. Area 3a receives vestibular and

somatosensory inputs and projects to area 4 of

the motor cortex. These connections likely serve

to integrate motor control of the head and body.

The Integrative Vestibular System

 By nature of the multiple connections with other

sensory inputs and motor output systems, the

vestibular system is a multisensory integrative

system, as has been noted previously. Vestibular

pathways are both unilateral and contralateral,

with recent evidence indicating fi bers cross at the

vestibular nuclei, in the pons, the midbrain, and

at the corpus callosum ( Kirsch et al., 2014 ). The

bilaterality of this system is considered crucial

to its function ( Rine & Wiener-Vacher, 2013 ).

According to Dieterich and Brandt ( 2015 ), vestibular inputs are multimodal, activating other

sensory systems as they are used to mediate our

perception of body position in and movement

through space, as well as gaze stabilization.

Unique among sensory systems, the vestibular

system has no primary sensory cortex; regions

of the cortex receiving vestibular inputs (i.e.,

the parietal-insular area) also respond to other

sensory inputs. The vestibular system then uses

multimodal and multilevel integration in conducting its critical functions.

Vestibular and Proprioception

Interactions

 Vestibular and proprioceptive processing are

hypothesized to contribute jointly to the perception of active movement, the development of

body scheme, and the development and use of

postural responses—especially those involving

extensor muscles (e.g., extensor muscle tone,

equilibrium). Reviewing available research at

the time, Matthews ( 1988 ) indicated that, under

typical circumstances, the role of proprioception

was to provide the motor system with a clear and

unambiguous map of the external environment

and of the body. Other investigators of the time

had suggested that proprioception played a role in

programming and planning of bilateral projected

action sequences ( Goldberg, 1985 ). Nashner

 ( 1982 ) had already proposed that inputs from

the vestibular system could be used to resolve

vestibular-visual-somatosensory (proprioceptive)

confl icts, and, as such, the two systems worked

together to provide a stable frame of reference

against which other sensory inputs were interpreted. These early indications of function have

remained with us ( Goldberg et al., 2012 ). Thus,

vestibular and proprioceptive inputs, together

with vision, provide:

• Subjective awareness and coordination of

movement of the head in space

• Postural tone and equilibrium

• Coordination of the eyes, head, and body, and

stabilization of the eyes in space during head

movements (compensatory eye movements)

HERE ’ S THE POINT

 • Peripheral vestibular receptors (hair cells) in

the inner ear respond to linear (otoliths) and

angular (semicircular canals) movement. All

movement input automatically infl uences

activity on both sides of the head.

 • The central vestibular system begins with the

brainstem nuclei, which themselves receive

multisensory inputs.

• Vestibular inputs to the cerebellum are both

direct, from the receptors, and indirect, by

way of vestibular nuclei, indicating the close

relationship these structures share in the

production of movement.

 • Projections from vestibular nuclei also include

fibers descending to regions of the spinal

cord and ascending to oculomotor nuclei, the

thalamus, and the cortex.

 • The vestibular system is a multisensory system

throughout its course; it is part of a network

involving vestibular nuclei, the cerebellum,

proprioception, spinal cord motor neurons,

oculomotor neurons, the thalamus, and the

cortex in supporting its functions related to

eye, head, and trunk control and body position

in and movement through space.

The Auditory System

 Activation of the auditory system is a complex

process; sound waves are received by the external

92 ■ PART II The Neuroscience Basis of Sensory Integration Disorders

ear, transmitted via the middle ear, and fi nally

transduced into action potentials within the inner

ear. The structure of the auditory system can be

seen situated in the temporal bone, adjacent to

the vestibular apparatus in Figure 4-20 . Receptors for the auditory system are located in the

inner ear, in a membranous structure called the

cochlea (see Fig. 4-21 ). The receptors are hair

cells, which are components of the organ of

Corti ( Fig. 4-23 ). As you may recall, hair cells

are also the receptors in the vestibular system,

and the mechanism of transduction from hair cell

receptors in the auditory system is similar to that

described for the vestibular system.

Receptors and Transduction

 Sound begins as sound waves, corralled by the

external ear and transmitted through the external auditory meatus to the tympanic membrane

( Fig. 4-23 ). Attached to the tympanic membrane

are the ossicles of the middle ear. The ossicles

act to optimize the transfer of sound energy from

air to the fl uid-fi lled inner ear, where the organ of

Corti lies. There are two muscles in the middle ear

that serve to alter responsivity of the ossicles to

movement of the tympanic membrane: the tensor

tympani and the stapedius. The tensor tympani

is attached to one ossicle (malleolus), and it is

innervated by a branch of the trigeminal nerve

(cranial nerve V). The actions linked with biting

and chewing lead to refl exive contraction of this

muscle, pulling on the malleus and stiffening the

tympanic membrane. This process effectively

prevents lower frequency sound (background

noise) from being transmitted and, therefore,

can improve our ability to understand speech

in the face of background noise. The stapedius

muscle attaches to the ossicle called the stapes;

 it is innervated by the facial nerve or cranial

nerve VII. The stapedius refl exively contracts

with sounds at a sound pressure level of 65 dB

and 500, 1,000, and 2,000 Hz, frequencies often

associated with vowel sounds in speech. Refl exive contraction of the stapedius acts in a manner

similar to that described previously, stiffening

the tympanic membrane and blocking lower frequency sounds from entering the ear. Because

of the relationship between the ossicles and the

tympanic membrane, diseases that impede movement of the ossicles decrease energy transfer and

interfere with hearing. This is what happens with

inner ear infections (e.g., otitis media).

 The transduction of sound into a neurochemical signal begins with movement of the tympanic membrane, creating a chain of events in

this closed system ( Fig. 4-23 ). Movement of

the tympanic membrane creates movement of

the ossicles, small bones, in the middle ear. The

malleus fi ts into the oval window, the opening to

FIGURE 4-23 A. The auditory system within the temporal bone. B. Cross section through the cochlea, showing

the three regions of the auditory canal. C. The organ of Corti, receptor cells for sound.

Helicotrema

of cochlea

Scala vestibuli

Cochlear duct

Scala tympani

Cochlear aqueduct

Round (cochlear) Vestibule

window

Stapes in oval

(vestibular) window

Incus

Malleus

Tympanic cavity

External acoustic meatus

Sound

Tympanic membrane

Tectorial

membrane

Basilar

membrane

Inner

hair cell

Outer

hair cell

Supporting

cell

Corti

tunnel

Scala vestibuli

(perilymph)

Scala tympani

(perilymph)

Cochlear duct

(endolymph)

Cochlear

nerve

Spiral

ganglion

Organ of

Corti

Spiral

ligament

Stria

vascularis

Vestibular

(Reissner)

membrane

Basilar

membrane

Eustachian

tube

Stereocilia

A

B C

CHAPTER 4 Structure and Function of the Sensory Systems ■ 93

the inner ear. Movement of the ossicles leads to

movement of the oval window. As the membrane

of the oval window moves in and out, it creates

waves of movement in the fl uid in the auditory

canal of the inner ear (perilymph). Within the

auditory canal is the basilar membrane; as the

perilymph moves, the basilar membrane also

moves. Sitting on the basilar membrane is the

organ of Corti, which contains hair cells. These

hair cells have projections into a second membrane, the tectorial membrane, which sits atop

hair cell projections. Movement of the basilar

membrane and hair cells results in bending of

the hair cell projections, beginning the process

of depolarization. The basilar membrane changes

thickness throughout its length, being thinner at

the base than at the apex. This makes it sensitive

to different frequencies (i.e., pitches) along its

length. The process from this point parallels that

in the vestibular system. Depolarization of the

hair cell releases a neurotransmitter (glutamate)

that interacts with receptor sites on the afferent

component of the auditory nerve, and information is carried to the CNS.

 Within the auditory system, the fi rst synapse

is very close to the point of transduction, at

the base of the hair cells. Activation of the hair

cells turns physical energy into electrical energy

and, almost immediately, chemical energy, as

the impulse is transferred to the dendrites of

the spiral ganglion, which synapse with the hair

cells. The organ of Corti includes two types of

hair cells, those thought to control the sensitivity of the receptor apparatus, the outer cells,

and those thought to be responsible primarily

for actual hearing, the inner hair cells. A single

spiral ganglion cell may innervate as many as

50 outer hair cells. In contrast, the inner hair cells

may receive dendritic connections from as many

as 10 spiral ganglion cells. Thus, whereas outer

hair cells converge, inner hair cells diverge at the

fi rst synapse in the pathway. The organ of Corti

is tonotopically organized such that high sounds

activate cells on the basilar membrane near its

narrow end, and low sounds activate cells at the

wide end of the membrane. In addition, inner

hair cells and the spiral ganglion cells synapsing

with them show a “tuning curve,” where there

is a relationship between the amplitude of sound

needed to induce a barely detectable neuronal

discharge and the sound frequency. These characteristics of the receptive apparatus account for

the accuracy with which sound information is

transmitted to the brain.

 As in the vestibular system, the auditory nerve

has both afferent and efferent components. The

afferent components form the cochlear portion of

the vestibular-cochlear nerve (cranial nerve VIII).

The efferents come from the superior olivary

complex (SOC) and innervate the outer hair cells

directly and the inner hair cells indirectly. When

active, the efferent fi bers inhibit transmission of

information to the CNS and may play a role in

the discrimination of specifi c sounds in the presence of background noise.

Central Connections

 The auditory system has two primary pathways

to the CNS: the core pathway, which maintains

tonotopic organization of input and transmits

sound frequency with speed and great accuracy,

and the belt pathway, which is less well organized and transmits information relative to the

timing and intensity of input. This latter pathway

contributes to bilateral interaction of sound input.

Information on both pathways is integrated into

the information that I will describe next. Core

and belt pathways project to primary (core) and

secondary (belt) cortical regions. Pathways and

cortical regions are described next.

 Axons from the spiral ganglion cells form the

cochlear nerve, which travels from the ear to the

brainstem where it synapses with ventral and

dorsal cochlear nuclei, ipsilaterally. All fi bers

have synapses in both nuclei. The tonotopic organization of these connections is maintained at this

level. From this point, three routes carry acoustic

information onward. From the dorsal cochlear

nucleus come fi bers that cross to become part of

the lateral lemniscus. From the ventral nucleus,

one group of fi bers follows those of the dorsal

nucleus, also becoming part of the lateral lemniscus. Another group passes to the ipsilateral and

contralateral nuclei of the trapezoid body and the

superior olivary nuclei and, from there, joins the

lateral lemniscus. Thus, the lateral lemniscus has

both ipsilateral and contralateral representation

of acoustic information, although contralateral

fi bers predominate. The superior olive is the fi rst

place where information from both ears converges. This convergence occurs with accurate

representation of the timing of auditory input to

the two ears, which is continued onto the cortex.

94 ■ PART II The Neuroscience Basis of Sensory Integration Disorders

Stimulus localization depends on the accurate

rendition of the temporal aspects of sound reception ( Fig. 4-24 ).

 Fibers within the lateral lemniscus travel

to the inferior colliculus and the medial geniculate body. The inferior colliculus receives

essentially all auditory input, both core and belt

pathway input, as well as input from the contralateral auditory cortex. As such, it is a major

integrating center for the auditory system. The

main nucleus of the inferior colliculus is the

central nucleus, where cells are sensitive to both

timing and intensity differences in the sound

transmissions received; this structure then functions in detecting both sound frequency and

sound temporal characteristics. Regions of the

inferior colliculus are also sensitive to differences in time of arrival of sound, giving it a

role in sound localization and binaural hearing

( Sahley & Musiek, 2015 ). Another site for auditory input in the inferior colliculus is the paracentral nucleus. This structure receives not only

auditory input but also inputs from the spinal

cord, dorsal column, and SC. This nucleus is

FIGURE 4-24 Central auditory pathways. Some auditory fi bers project in the inferior colliculus, whereas others

project to the medial geniculate nucleus of the thalamus and on to the auditory cortex.

Auditory

cortex

Section of

mesencephalon

Section of

brainstem

Thalamus

Inferior

colliculus

Reticular

formation

Reticular formation

Cochlear

nuclei

Superior

olive

Cochleo-vestibular

nerve (VIII)

Type 1 neuron

CHAPTER 4 Structure and Function of the Sensory Systems ■ 95

thought to play a role in multisensory integration

and auditory attention.

 The inferior colliculus sends fi bers on to the

medial geniculate body, a specialized nucleus

within the thalamus. From there, information

travels to the transverse temporal gyrus, also

called Heschl gyrus, occupying Brodmann areas

41 and 42 within the primary auditory cortex.

It receives input from the core pathway and is

tonotopically organized. The belt area surrounds

the area of core pathway input; it is both less

organized and less well understood. When information reaches the primary auditory cortex, a

sound is heard and interpretation begins. The

primary auditory cortex receives both ipsilateral and contralateral input, further supporting

the mapping of sound. Neurons in this area also

map loudness, modulation of loudness, and modulation of frequency ( Kandel, Schwartz, Jessell,

Siegelbaum, & Hudspeth, 2013 ). This cortical

region is critical for the perception of speech.

 Brodmann area 22 ( Fig. 4-7 ) corresponds to

the secondary auditory cortex, where discrimination of location and direction of sound take

place. The secondary auditory cortex receives

input from the paracentral nucleus of the inferior colliculus. As noted, this projection is likely

responsible for detecting and directing attention

to auditory input that is novel or moving. The

planum temporale is a part of this area, and it

is an area implicated in dyslexia. It is an area

that shows bilateral asymmetry, with somewhat

different functions for each side. The planum

temporale plays a role in processing complex

sound as well as processing basic auditory input

and speech ( Liem, Hurschler, Jancke, & Meyer,

 2014 ). Area 22 also receives input from the

visual and somatosensory pathways.

 The auditory association cortex encompasses

areas 39 and 40 ( Fig. 4-7 ), the angular gyrus and

supramarginal gyrus, respectively. These areas are

associated with reading and writing. Damage to

area 39 leads to an inability to recognize speech.

Projections from the primary auditory cortex are

also found in other cortical regions associated

with speech. Areas 44 and 45 have been called

the Broca area; damage in that area results in

speech that is nonfl uent, although speech recognition is not impaired by such damage. The association area of the auditory cortex also receives

input from other systems, such as the vestibular and somatosensory systems. Thus, there is

multisensory interaction here, and this may play

a role in arousal or attention.

Efferent Processes and Feedback Loops

 Within the auditory pathways are numerous

efferent processes thought to act as feedback

loops. Functionally, they may contribute to

selective auditory attention. Reticulospinal pathways “sample” activity in the lateral lemniscus

and play a role in auditory startle responses. In

addition, the inferior colliculus and the auditory

cortex project to the SC, where information is

integrated with somatosensory inputs. These

pathways are likely responsible for controlling

orientation of the head, eyes, and body to sound.

HERE ’ S THE POINT

 • Auditory receptors (hair cells), found in the

organ of Corti in the inner ear, are activated

through a series of events that begin when

sound waves create movement of the

tympanic membrane separating the outer

and middle ear.

 • Sound input is projected to brainstem nuclei,

and from there information from both ears

is projected to the superior olive. The receipt

of bilateral auditory input allows for temporal

interpretation of sound, and sound localization.

 • Multisensory integration (somatosensory,

visual, and auditory) takes place in the inferior

colliculus and supports auditory attention.

 • Projections of auditory input to the primary and

secondary auditory cortices subserve a higher

level of sound interpretation and complex

mapping of the sound environment.

 • Additional cortical projections, some to

multisensory integration areas, are associated

with such functions as sound interpretation,

auditory attention, speech, reading, and

writing.

The Visual System

 Despite the pervasive nature of the tactile system

and the importance of the vestibular system,

we rely most heavily for day-to-day function

on visual input. According to one neuroscience

text, “It is vision that helps us to navigate in the

world to judge the speed and distance of objects; 



sensory integration disorder

sensory integration

sensory integration therapy

sensory integration dysfunction

sensory integration therapy near me

sensory integration frame of reference

sensory integration and praxis test

sensory integration theory

sensory integration disorder icd 10

sensory integration and the child

sensory integration activities

sensory integration assessment

sensory integration autism

sensory integration and the child pdf

sensory integration and praxis test (sipt)

sensory integration adults

sensory integration activities for adults

sensory integration approach

sensory integration books

sensory integration brushing

sensory integration brain

sensory integration billing code

sensory integration benefits

sensory integration book pdf

sensory integration balance training

sensory integration behavior problems

sensory integration books for occupational therapists

sensory integration by jean ayres

sensory integration certification

sensory integration clinic

sensory integration clinic boston

sensory integration cpt code

sensory integration courses

sensory integration challenges

sensory integration center

sensory integration ceu courses

sensory integration clinic near me

sensory integration catalog

sensory integration disorder symptoms

sensory integration definition

sensory integration disorder in adults

sensory integration disorder treatment

sensory integration disorder dsm 5

sensory integration disorder autism

sensory integration dysfunction icd 10

sensory integration examples

sensory integration evaluation

sensory integration exercises

sensory integration equipment

sensory integration exercises for adults

sensory integration education

sensory integration early childhood

sensory integration early intervention

sensory integration emotional regulation

sensory integration evidence based practice

sensory integration for adults

sensory integration for

sensory integration for autism

sensory integration fidelity measure

sensory integration for adhd

sensory integration for dementia

sensory integration for occupational therapy

sensory integration fact sheet aota

sensory integration for anxiety

sensory integration goal bank

sensory integration gym

sensory integration goals

sensory integration global network

sensory integration google scholar

sensory integration group activities

sensory integration guide for parents

what is the meaning of sensory integration

does sensory integration work

why sensory integration is important

sensory integration handout for parents

sensory integration heavy work

sensory integration hyposensitivity

sensory integration hypersensitivity

sensory integration head banging

sensory integration history

sensory integration hierarchy

sensory integration hk

sensory integration hammocks

sensory integration help

sensory integration issues

sensory integration interventions

sensory integration is critical to the development of

sensory integration inventory

sensory integration in the classroom

sensory integration interventions occupational therapy

sensory integration interventions examples

sensory integration icd 10

sensory integration involves

sensory integration inventory revised

sensory integration jean ayres

sensory integration jobs

sensory integration jean ayres pdf

sensory integration jobs uk

sensory integration journal article

sensory integration journal

sensory integration ترجمة

sensory processing jobs

sensory processing jumping

sensory integration therapy jobs

sensory integration kindergarten

sensory integration kit

sensory integration kinesthesia

sensory integration key role

sensory processing kinesthesia

sensory integration therapy kolkata

sensory integration therapy kent

sensory integration center kota kinabalu

sensory integration therapy katy

sensory integration disorder kindergarten

sensory integration level 1

sensory integration login

sensory integration level 2

sensory integration levels

sensory integration level 3

sensory integration learning disabilities

sensory integration london

sensory integration learning

sensory integration level 4

sensory integration low registration

sensory integration model

sensory integration meaning

sensory integration massage

sensory integration music therapy

sensory integration milestones

sensory integration masters

sensory integration module 1

sensory integration meaning in hindi

sensory integration materials

sensory integration module 2

sensory integration near me

sensory integration network

sensory integration needs

sensory integration network login

sensory integration nhs

sensory integration network jobs

sensory integration neurodiversity

sensory integration neuroscience

sensory integration network find a therapist

sensory integration network courses

sensory integration occupational therapy

sensory integration ot

sensory integration ot near me

sensory integration occupational therapy interventions

sensory integration occupational therapy near me

sensory integration occupational therapy activities

sensory integration occupational therapy goals

sensory integration ot theory

sensory integration observation worksheet

sensory integration occupational therapy assessment

sensory integration praxis test

sensory integration problems

sensory integration processing disorder

sensory integration pdf

sensory integration psychology

sensory integration proprioception

sensory integration pediatric occupational therapy

sensory integration pediatrics

sensory integration principles

sensory integration play

sensory integration questionnaire

sensory integration quizlet

sensory integration quotes

sensory integration questions

sensory integration quadrants

sensory integration quiz

sensory integration qualification

sensory processing questionnaire pdf

sensory processing quadrants

sensory processing quotes

sensory integration research

sensory integration room

sensory integration resources

sensory integration room design

sensory integration room equipment

sensory integration room ideas

sensory integration register

sensory integration report

sensory integration research articles

sensory integration rcot

sensory integration strategies

sensory integration specialist

sensory integration skills

sensory integration speech therapy

sensory integration symptoms

sensory integration swings

sensory integration syndrome

sensory integration screening questionnaire

sensory integration supplies

sensory integration systems

sensory integration training

sensory integration therapy for adults

sensory integration therapy at home

sensory integration theory and practice

sensory integration techniques

sensory integration therapy for autism

sensory integration therapy for adults near me

sensory integration usc

sensory integration uk

sensory integration ulster university

sensory integration ulster

sensory processing uk

sensory processing under responsive

sensory processing under sensitivity

sensory processing underwear

sensory processing understood

sensory processing unit

sensory integration vs sensory processing

sensory integration vs modulation

sensory integration vestibular

sensory integration video

sensory integration visual activities

sensory integration vestibular activities

sensory integration vestibular dysfunction

sensory integration vision

sensory integration vs aba

sensory integration vestibular proprioception

sensory integration webinar

sensory integration with diverse populations

sensory integration with autism

sensory integration what is it

sensory integration workshop

sensory integration wikipedia

sensory integration weighted blanket

sensory integration work

sensory integration with adults

sensory integration with older adults

sensory integration xyz

sensory integration xi

sensory integration xray

sensory integration yoga

sensory integration youtube

sensory integration york

sensory processing youtube

multisensory integration youtube

sensory integration 2 year old

sensory integration therapy youtube

sensory integration and yoga course

sensory integration disorder 3 year old

sensory integration zebrafish

sensory integration zhongwen

sensory processing zone out

zoloft sensory integration disorder

what's sensory integration

example of sensory integration

best sensory integration books

best sensory activities for autism

best sensory swing for autism

best sensory items for autism

best sensory apps for ipad

best bean bag chairs for sensory integration

best books for sensory integration

how to improve sensory integration

is sensory integration evidence based

best sensory swing for adults

best sensory items for adults

best sensory objects for autism

best sensory kits for toddlers

best sensory toys for kindergarten

best sensory swing for toddlers

best sensory toys for asd

best sensory room ideas

best sensory room items

best sensory rooms

best sensory swing indoor

best sensory videos for newborns

best sensory bins for autism

best sensory items for toddlers

sensory integration for asd

sensory integration for adults with autism

sensory integration for adults with learning disabilities

sensory integration for autistic toddlers

benefits of sensory integration for autism

sensory integration for babies

sensory integration for blind child

sensory integration for balance

brushing techniques for sensory integration

therapy ball for sensory integration

bean bag chair for sensory integration

building blocks for sensory integration

modified clinical test for sensory integration and balance

sensory integration for cerebral palsy

sensory integration for pc

sensory integration for chronic pain

icd 10 code for sensory integration disorder

sensory integration activities for the classroom

sensory integration courses for occupational therapists

icd-10 code for sensory integration disorder of childhood

clinician's guide for implementing ayres sensory integration

sensory integration for down syndrome

sensory integration for dogs

sensory integration for dummies

sensory integration for dyspraxia

icd 10 for sensory integration disorder

sensory integration inventory for individuals with developmental disabilities

different textures for sensory integration

sensory integration therapy for down syndrome

sensory integration for elderly

examples of occupational therapy goals for sensory integration

evidence for sensory integration therapy

suspended equipment for sensory integration

sensory integration explained for parents

sensory integration therapy for elderly

equipment for sensory integration therapy

exercises for sensory integration

vestibular exercises for sensory integration

sensory integration frame of reference for adults

a frame of reference for sensory integration

sensory integration for hand flapping

swing frame for sensory integration

what are sensory integration techniques

sensory integration for geriatrics

sensory integration a guide for preschool teachers

gum chewing for sensory integration

sample occupational therapy goals for sensory integration

song games for sensory integration

sample iep goals for sensory integration

ot goals for sensory integration disorder

sensory integration for hyperactivity

sensory integration for human balance control

the integration site for sensory and motor information in humans is the

sensory integration for mental health

home program for sensory integration

heavy work for sensory integration

how do you test for sensory integration disorder

occupational therapy for kids/specializing in sensory integration

occupational therapy interventions for sensory integration

what is sensory integration therapy for autism

south african institute for sensory integration

interventions for sensory integration

what part of the brain is responsible for sensory integration

clinical test for sensory integration in balance

what is a weighted vest for sensory integration

joint compression for sensory integration

jumping for sensory integration

sensory integration training for speech and language therapists

lap weight for sensory integration

perceptual level for sensory integration

lycra for sensory integration

sercri solutions for sensory integration lda

sensory integration for middle school students

sensory integration for motor planning

areas for interpretation and integration of most sensory information

outcome measures for sensory integration

sensory integration activities for mouth

materials for sensory integration

sensory integration for neuropathy

sensory integration for special needs

other name for sensory integration disorder

another name for sensory integration disorder

sensory integration for ot

sensory integration training for occupational therapists

sensory integration activities for 3 year old

sensory integration for parents

sensory integration for preschool

sensory integration for process

sensory integration therapy for autism pdf

brushing protocol for sensory integration

sensory integration and praxis test for adults

vibration plate for sensory integration

sensory integration for stroke patients

sensory integration questionnaire for parents

the thalamus is responsible for the integration of sensory information

primitive reflexes for sensory integration

reasons for sensory integration disorder

reflex for sensory integration

sensory integration for speech therapy

sensory integration for schizophrenia

swings for sensory integration

sensory integration therapy for schizophrenia

spinning for sensory integration

sensory integration for teachers

sensory integration for trauma

sensory integration for toddlers

sensory integration for therapist

sensory integration training for teachers

sensory integration training for occupational therapists uk

what is sensory integration therapy used for

sensory integration therapy used for autism

what to use for sensory integration disorder

sensory integration therapy for cerebral palsy usa

sensory integration for vestibular

sensory integration for visual impairments

occupational therapy and sensory integration for visual impairment

vibration for sensory integration

vestibular activities for sensory integration

pressure vest for sensory integration

ankle weights for sensory integration

sensory integration therapy for adults with autism

the wilbarger protocol (brushing) for sensory integration

sensory integration therapy for child with autism

sensory integration therapy for adults with dementia

what is the icd 10 code for sensory integration disorder

yoga for sensory integration

what do you do for sensory integration dysfunction

sensory integration ideas

sensory integration for infants

best sensory

what are the best sensory toys for autism

intensive sensory integration therapy

best sensory products

best sensory items

benefits of sensory integration

integration of senses

pros and cons of sensory integration therapy

types of sensory integration therapy

g-sensor sensitivity

best indoor sensory swing

best sensory kits

best proprioceptive toys

sensory integration programs

best sensory room equipment

best sensory integration therapy

best sensory integration courses

best sensory integration tools

sensory-integration therapy

best x axis sensitivity fortnite

best x and y sens fortnite

best z-wave motion sensor

best z-wave sensors

adults with sensory integration disorder

adhd with sensory integration disorder

a child with sensory integration disorder

sensory integration is an evidence-based practice for students with disabilities

how to help child with sensory integration disorder

child with sensory integration disorder

parenting a child with sensory integration disorder

child with sensory integration difficulties

understanding the nature of sensory integration with diverse populations

sensory integration and the perceptual experience of persons with autism

effects of occupational therapy with sensory integration

effects of occupational therapy with sensory integration emphasis

what is sensory integration dysfunction

sensory integration inventory for adults with developmental disabilities

sensory integration therapy for students with autism

how to get diagnosed with sensory integration disorder

how occupational therapy helps with sensory integration issues

activities to help with sensory integration

toys to help with sensory integration

how to work with sensory integration disorder

help with sensory integration

helping child with sensory integration

issues with sensory integration

sensory integration jane ayres

sensory integration theory jean ayres

sensory integration methods

sensory integration with occupational therapy

ot with sensory integration

sensory integration therapy with ot

problems with sensory integration

patients with sensory integration dysfunction

sensory and reflex integration

sensory integration primitive reflexes

sensory integration disorder without autism

sensory integration vs sensory modulation

occupational therapy with sensory integration

sensory integration explained

sensory play with yogurt

sensory enrichment zoo tycoon

sensory friendly zoo lights

sensory processing without autism

sensory disorder without autism

sensory deprivation without tank

sensory issues without autism

sensory integration therapy not evidence based

sensory overload without autism

sensory processing disorder without autism

sensory integration frame of reference occupational therapy

sensory integration therapy for adhd

sensory integration and praxis tests (sipt)

sensory integration clinical observation

sensory integration and praxis tests

sensory integration and trauma

sensory integration ayres

sensory integration activities for autism

sensory integration aba

sensory integration and the child 25th anniversary edition

sensory integration activities for toddlers

sensory integration blessings

sensory integration babies

sensory integration bundy

sensory integration book ayres

sensory integration brushing teeth

sensory processing brain

sensory integration ceu

sensory integration classroom

sensory integration classes

sensory integration child

sensory integration calming techniques

sensory integration cerebral palsy

sensory integration conference

sensory integration clasi

sensory processing cup analogy

sensory processing cartoon

sensory processing cup

sensory integration diet

sensory integration disorder in toddlers

sensory integration disorder vs autism

sensory integration deep pressure

sensory integration delay

sensory processing disorder

sensory processing disorder toddler

sensory processing disorder simulation

sensory processing disorder vs autism

sensory processing disorder in hindi

sensory processing disorder explained

sensory processing disorder tiktok

sensory integration eating

sensory integration evidence

sensory processing explained

sensory integration therapy exercises

sensory integration feeding therapy

sensory integration frame of reference ot

sensory integration food aversions

sensory integration frame

sensory integration feeding

sensory processing from a child's perspective

sensory integration in hindi

sensory integration intervention

sensory integration in school setting

sensory integration ideas for the classroom

sensory integration in speech therapy

sensory integration in the brain

sensory integration in preschool classroom

sensory integration in the child

sensory integration in ot

sensory integration intensive

sensory integration in cerebral palsy

sensory integration lecture

sensory integration motor

sensory integration mouth

sensory integration module 3

sensory integration module 4

sensory motor integration techniques

sensory processing measure

sensory processing measure scoring

sensory processing measure 2

ayres sensory integration and neuroscience

sensory integration olfactory activities

sensory integration outcome measures

sensory integration online course

sensory integration obstacle course

sensory processing occupational therapy

sensory processing overload

sensory processing over responsive

neural integration sensory input

sensory integration physical therapy

sensory integration pyramid of learning

sensory integration practitioner

sensory integration pyramid

sensory integration products

sensory integration premature babies

sensory integration programme

sensory integration process

sensory integration proprioceptive activities

sensory integration powerpoint

sensory integration pathway

sensory integration therapy room

sensory integration session

sensory integration strategies for the classroom

sensory integration screening

sensory integration senses

sensory integration smell

sensory integration sound sensitivity

sensory integration systematic review

sensory integration swing activities

sensory integration speech and language therapy

sensory processing sensitivity

sensory processing sensitivity (sps)

sensory integration therapy for cerebral palsy

sensory integration therapy for sound

sensory integration tactile

sensory integration therapy for autism in tamil

sensory integration therapy in hindi

sensory integration therapy for autism in hindi

sensory integration usmle

sensory integration ultrasound

sensory processing video

sensory integration xbox

sensory integration zen

sensory integration zoo

sensory integration zoom

sensory integration zen den

sensory integration zakir naik


Comments

Search This Blog

Archive

Show more

Popular posts from this blog

TRIPASS XR تري باس

CELEPHI 200 MG, Gélule

ZENOXIA 15 MG, Comprimé

VOXCIB 200 MG, Gélule

Kana Brax Laberax

فومي كايند

بعض الادويه نجد رموز عليها مثل IR ، MR, XR, CR, SR , DS ماذا تعني هذه الرموز

NIFLURIL 700 MG, Suppositoire adulte

Antifongiques مضادات الفطريات

Popular posts from this blog

علاقة البيبي بالفراولة بالالفا فيتو بروتين

التغيرات الخمس التي تحدث للجسم عند المشي

إحصائيات سنة 2020 | تعداد سكَان دول إفريقيا تنازليا :

ما هو الليمونير للأسنان ؟

ACUPAN 20 MG, Solution injectable

CELEPHI 200 MG, Gélule

الام الظهر

VOXCIB 200 MG, Gélule

ميبستان

Popular posts from this blog

TRIPASS XR تري باس

CELEPHI 200 MG, Gélule

Popular posts from this blog

TRIPASS XR تري باس

CELEPHI 200 MG, Gélule

ZENOXIA 15 MG, Comprimé

VOXCIB 200 MG, Gélule

Kana Brax Laberax

فومي كايند

بعض الادويه نجد رموز عليها مثل IR ، MR, XR, CR, SR , DS ماذا تعني هذه الرموز

NIFLURIL 700 MG, Suppositoire adulte

Antifongiques مضادات الفطريات

Popular posts from this blog

Kana Brax Laberax

TRIPASS XR تري باس

PARANTAL 100 MG, Suppositoire بارانتال 100 مجم تحاميل

الكبد الدهني Fatty Liver

الم اسفل الظهر (الحاد) الذي يظهر بشكل مفاجئ bal-agrisi

SEDALGIC 37.5 MG / 325 MG, Comprimé pelliculé [P] سيدالجيك 37.5 مجم / 325 مجم ، قرص مغلف [P]

نمـو الدمـاغ والتطـور العقـلي لـدى الطفـل

CELEPHI 200 MG, Gélule

أخطر أنواع المخدرات فى العالم و الشرق الاوسط

Archive

Show more